Skip to main content

Role of milk and dairy intake in cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Abstract

Background

As aging populations increase across the globe, research on lifestyle factors that prevent cognitive decline and dementia is urgently needed. Therefore, a systematic review was conducted to examine the effects of varying levels of milk intake alone or in combination with other dairy products on the outcomes of cognitive function and disorders in adults.

Methods

A comprehensive search was conducted across 3 databases (PUBMED, CINAHL, and EMBASE) from their inception through October 2017. Prospective cohort studies and randomized controlled trials (RCTs) that enrolled adults were included. Studies with follow-up durations of less than 4 weeks and studies including schizophrenic patients were excluded. Two independent investigators conducted abstract and full-text screenings, data extractions, and risk-of-bias (ROB) assessments using validated tools. Studies were synthesized qualitatively using a strength of evidence (SoE) rating tool. A random-effects model for meta-analysis was conducted when at least 3 unique studies reported sufficient quantitative data for the same outcome.

Results

A total of 1 RCT and 7 cohort studies were included. One medium-quality small RCT (n = 38 participants) showed that only spatial working memory was marginally better in the high dairy diet group compared to the low dairy diet group. Two of the 7 cohort studies were rated as having a high ROB, and only 1 cohort study was rated as having a low ROB. There were large methodological and clinical heterogeneities, such as the methods used to assess milk or dairy intake and the characteristics of the study populations. It was impossible to conduct a dose-response meta-analysis because the studies utilized different categories of exposures (e.g., different frequencies of milk consumption or the amount of dairy intake). Thus, the overall SoE was rated as insufficient regarding the associations between milk intake and cognitive decline, dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease outcomes. Our meta-analysis of 3 cohort studies showed no significant association between milk intake and cognitive decline outcome (pooled adjusted risk ratio = 1.21; 95% CI: 0.81, 1.82; for highest vs. lowest intake) with large statistical heterogeneity (I2 = 64.1%).

Conclusions

The existing evidence (mostly observational) is too poor to draw a firm conclusion regarding the effect of milk or dairy intake on the risk of cognitive decline or disorders in adults.

Peer Review reports

Background

The prevention of cognitive decline and dementia is an increasingly important public health priority due to the growth of the global elderly population [1]. The global prevalence of dementia has been rising, and the number of people with dementia is projected to total 81.1 million by 2040 [2]. Dementia, the most severe cognitive disorder, not only negatively impacts the patients’ quality of life but also creates a substantial burden for caregivers [3, 4].

Cognitive decline is a precursor to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and is potentially the earliest clinical indicator of dementia [5, 6]. Most patients experience a subjective cognitive decline, also called subjective memory complaint, before noticeable cognitive impairment [7, 8]. Subjective Cognitive Decline (SCD) refers to a self-experienced persistent decline in cognitive abilities in comparison with a prior normal status and independent of the objective performance on neuropsychological tests [9]. By definition, SCD is a sign of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and can occur before objective cognitive impairment appears. SCD can be diagnosed by several questionnaires of self-reported cognitive performance, such as the Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE) and the Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) without the use of neuropsychological tests for objective assessment of cognitive function [10]. Currently, there is no ‘gold standard’ measure for SCD [11]. The international SCD Initiative Working Group has systematically identified 34 self-report SCD measures and has found wide variations in the definitions, cognitive domains, optimal items for each domain, item response options, and time frame across measures [12, 13].

SCD is a potential marker for future Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). Currently, MCI is diagnosed by one of three criteria: the revised Mayo Clinic Criteria, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5), or the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroup (NIA-AA) [14]. Among the three diagnostic criteria, one commonly shared core characteristic is the objective evidence of impairment from standardized neuropsychological tests in ≥1 cognitive domains (i.e., memory, executive function, attention, language, and visuospatial skills) [14]. Although there is no gold standard regarding neuropsychological tests, it is critical to investigate all major cognitive domains for objective cognitive impairment. Dementia [15], including Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia, can be detected by objective measures of cognitive impairment and biomarkers, such as β-amyloid (1–42), total tau, and phospho-tau-181 in cerebrospinal fluid [16].

Milk and dairy products are recommended by many dietary guidelines for meeting the daily requirements for calcium, protein, and vitamin B12 intake. These nutrients are important for maintaining good health in older adults. The biological mechanisms linking milk or dairy consumption to cognitive function are not fully understood. It has been postulated that phospholipids in the milk fat globule membrane (MFGM) might affect cognitive function [17]. There are several possible reasons for why the intake of MFGM could benefit cognitive function [18]. First, MFGM contains high levels of choline derivatives (i.e., phosphocholine, glycerophosphocholine, phosphatidylcholine and sphingomyelin) [19]. These compounds may play an important role in the development of the nervous system. Second, sphingomyelin metabolites are essential elements of the myelin sheath that covers the axons of neurons. Therefore, sphingomyelin metabolites support the myelination and production of neurotransmitters in the brain. Additionally, previous studies have suggested that dietary phospholipids are effective transporters of essential fatty acids that could improve brain health by lowering endoplasmic reticulum stress [20], which is known to increase the risk of neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease. Lastly, the solubility of phospholipids in brain cell membranes may enhance the neuroplasticity of the hippocampus and support dopamine and glutamate transmission [19].

A previous meta-analysis that examined the potential relationship between milk consumption (with or without other dairy products) and cognitive function or disorders showed that the highest level of milk intake compared to the lowest intake level (as defined by the original studies) was significantly associated with a lower risk of cognitive disorders (pooled odds ratio [OR] = 0.72; 95% CI: 0.56–0.93; I2 = 64%) [21]. However, this meta-analysis pooled cohort studies and cross-sectional studies together and did not consider the risk-of-bias across the studies included in the meta-analysis as per the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses) guideline [22]. A risk-of-bias assessment can help explain variation (heterogeneity) in the results of studies included in a systematic review or meta-analysis, in that more rigorous studies are more likely to yield results that are closer to the truth. The potential limitations of the included studies must be carefully considered in the evidence synthesis in order to obtain reliable conclusions. Therefore, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis following the rigorous methods outlined in the Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews to evaluate whether there is a causal relationship between milk intake and cognitive function or dementia.

Methods

Identification of studies and study eligibility criteria

The search strategy was developed based on the search strategy used in an earlier meta-analysis by Wu and Sun [21]. The searches were carried out in 3 databases: PubMed (from inception to September 18, 2017), CINAHL (from inception to October 12, 2017) and EMBASE (from inception to October 12, 2017) and were limited to human studies without language restrictions. The complete search strategy is presented in Additional file 1: Table S1. In addition to the citations identified in our searches, all studies included in Wu and Sun’s meta-analysis were also evaluated based on the eligibility criteria of the present study.

For the present systematic review, prospective cohort studies and intervention trials with follow-up durations of longer than 4 weeks in adults, aged 18 years or older, were included. To be included, studies must have compared varying doses of milk intake, alone or in combination with other dairy products (i.e., yogurt and cheese), and reported outcomes related to cognitive function, including any stage of dementia (i.e., cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairments, and dementia) or any type of dementia (i.e., Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia). Studies including schizophrenic patients or those that measured only isolated specific nutrients in milk were excluded.

Study selection process

All citations identified from the literature searches were independently screened by at least two investigators, according to the pre-established screening criteria to exclude irrelevant abstracts (e.g., animal, in vitro, and cross-sectional studies). The abstract screening was performed using an open-source, online software—Abstrackr [23]. Full-text screening was independently executed by two investigators based on the final study eligibility criteria. Conflicts regarding both the abstract and full-text screenings were resolved by group consensus.

Data extraction, and quality assessment

Two independent investigators extracted data from each included study using the standardized data extraction forms. Discrepancies were resolved between the two investigators. The risk of bias (i.e., quality) of each included study was assessed using validated tools. For prospective cohort studies, we adapted the Newcastle Ottawa Scale (NOS) [24]. The modified NOS tool included quality items regarding potential selection bias, comparability of the comparison groups (e.g., potential for confounding bias), adequate sample size (e.g., power calculation), and potential biases in outcome assessments and selective outcome reporting. The response options for each quality item were high, unclear, or low risk, with detailed instructions on how to reach these judgements (Additional file 1: Table S2). When five or more items were rated as having a high or unclear risk, the overall risk of bias (ROB) was rated as high. When less than two of the quality items were rated as having a high or unclear risk, the overall ROB was rated as low. These cutoffs for rating the overall ROB were arbitrary, as the NOS did not provide guidance for overall ROB rating. For intervention trials, we used the revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for the specific intervention trial designs [25]. This ROB tool includes quality items regarding potential bias arising from the randomization process, bias due to deviations from the intended intervention, bias due to missing outcome data, bias in outcome measurement, and bias in selective outcome reporting. The suggested algorithms for reaching ROB judgments, as specified in the tool manual, were followed [25].

Qualitative synthesis

All the included studies were qualitatively synthesized in narrative form and in summary tables that tabulated the key features of the study populations, study designs, interventions or exposures, outcomes, and results. The strength of evidence (SoE) for major comparisons and outcomes was assessed through a consensus process of the entire research team, using an evidence grading system utilized by the American Diabetes Association and other prominent groups [26, 27]. Briefly, for each outcome, the SoE level was rated as A (Strong), B (Moderate), C (Limited), D (Inadequate), E (Expert Consensus or Clinical Experience), or NA (Not Applicable). Further details can be found in Additional file 1: Table S3.

Quantitative synthesis

In light of large clinical heterogeneity (e.g., different outcome measures and various milk intake exposures), a random-effects meta-analysis was performed when there were at least three unique studies that reported sufficient quantitative data for the same outcome [28]. After careful examination of all the extracted quantitative data and heterogeneity issues, only prospective cohort studies reporting the associations between milk intake and cognitive decline outcomes could be pooled. Further, a meta-analysis comparing only the highest and the lowest milk intake level was conducted, because data were insufficient to conduct a dose-response meta-regression. Both the Q statistic (considered significant when the P value was less than 0.10) and the I2 index were used to quantify the extent of statistical heterogeneity [29]. I2 values of 25%, 50% and 75% were defined as low, moderate and high heterogeneity, respectively. However, these cutoffs are arbitrary and were used for descriptive purposes only [30]. All calculations and meta-analyses were conducted in Stata SE 14 (Stata Corp). The analytical dataset can be found in the Additional file 2. Two-tailed P values less than 0.05 were considered significant.

Results

Our literature search yielded a total of 2407 citations. Of these, 1777 unduplicated abstracts were identified for the dual abstract screening. Further, we screened 29 potentially relevant full-text articles and finally included 8 articles (1 RCT and 7 prospective cohort studies). Figure 1 summarizes the details of the literature search and selection process. The characteristics of the included studies are summarized in Tables 1 and 2. In this paper, we organized results by research questions, namely the effects of milk intake on cognitive function, cognitive decline/impairment, and dementia. One RCT examined only cognitive function outcomes, and the results of this RCT were summarized first, before the results of cohort studies.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Literature search and study selection process

Table 1 Characteristics of the included RCT studiesa
Table 2 Characteristics of the included cohort studiesa

Milk and dairy intake and cognitive function

RCTs

Only one crossover RCT [31] met the inclusion criteria (Table 1). Participants (n = 38) were randomized to either a high-dairy diet (four servings of reduced-fat dairy foods per day) or the low-dairy diet (one serving of reduced-fat dairy foods per day) for 6 months, followed by an alternate diet switch for another 6 months, without a washout period. Nine indicators were used to measure cognitive performance: 1) verbal memory, 2) processing speed, 3) working memory, 4) visual attention, 5) verbal fluency, 6) abstract reasoning, 7) selective reasoning, 8) executive function, and 9) psychological well-being. Among these cognitive function outcomes, only backward spatial span in working memory showed a significant difference between the high-dairy and low-dairy diet groups (mean ± SEM; 7.9 ± 0.4 vs. 7.3 ± 0.4, P = 0.046 [31]) (Table 1). The overall ROB of this crossover RCT was rated as moderate due to the high risk for selective outcome reporting bias, some concerns regarding bias due to deviations from intended interventions and bias in outcome measurements, and no information regarding the randomization process (Table 4).

Cohort studies

Two cohort studies [32, 33] investigated the associations between milk or dairy intake and cognitive functions among older male and female adults after 5 to 20 years of follow-up in France and in the U.S. (Table 2). One cohort study assessed milk intake using a food-frequency questionnaire (FFQ) [33], and the other evaluated total dairy and milk intake using a 24-h recall [32]. The two cohort studies utilized the following assessment tools to assess cognitive function: DWRT (Delayed Word Recall Test), DSST (Digital Symbol Substitution Test), WFT (Word Fluency Test), RI-48 test, and the Delis-KapLan Trail Making Test. Both studies showed mixed results regarding the associations between milk intake and a variety of cognitive function outcomes. Specifically, the first cohort study showed no significant association between total dairy intake and cognitive function (i.e., working memory and verbal memory [32]). This study indicated no significant association between milk intake and working memory, while a higher milk intake was associated with poorer verbal memory performance [32]. The other study showed no significant association between milk intake and verbal learning, short-term memory, executive function or expressive language but found that a higher milk intake was significantly negatively associated with executive function [33] (Table 3). The overall ROB was rated as moderate for both studies, primarily due to inadequate methods used to ascertain exposure, unclear risk for statistical power and high risk for biased outcome assessment methods (Table 4).

Table 3 Results of the prospective cohort studiesa
Table 4 Risk of bias assessment for the included cohort studiesa

Milk and dairy intake and cognitive decline or impairment

Three cohort studies [34,35,36] reported the associations between milk or dairy intake and cognitive decline or cognitive impairment among elderly participants after 4.8 to 13 years of follow-up (Table 2). Milk or dairy intake was assessed by FFQ or diet history questionnaire. One of these studies compared a ‘regular’ consumption group with a ‘rare’ consumption group of full-cream milk [36], while the other two studies investigated the associations between cognitive decline/impairments and tertiles of milk intake [34, 35]. Cognitive impairment or decline was assessed using the MMSE (Mini-Mental State Examination), DECO (‘DEtérioration Cognitive Observée’, observed cognitive deterioration) and IADL (Iinstrumental Activities of Daily Living). One cohort study found that the regular full-cream milk consumption group demonstrated a significant decrease in successful mental health aging compared with the rare consumption group (adjusted hazard ratio = 0.63; 95% CI: 0.45, 0.89) [36]. The other two studies found no significant associations between milk and dairy consumption and cognitive decline [34, 35] (Table 3). Our random-effects meta-analysis results did not show significant differences in risk for cognitive decline or cognitive impairment by comparing the highest milk intake to lowest intake groups (pooled adjusted risk ratio = 1.21; 95% CI: 0.81, 1.82), with large statistical heterogeneity (I2 = 64.1%) (Fig. 2). Overall, the ROB was rated as being moderate or high, mainly due to high loss of follow-up (> 20%) and inadequate methods used to ascertain exposure (Table 4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Summary associations between milk intake and cognitive decline

Milk and dairy intake and Alzheimer’s disease

Two cohort studies [37, 38] examined the associations between milk or dairy intake and Alzheimer’s disease among elderly participants [38] and adult men and women [37] after 17 and 22 years of follow-up. Milk or dairy intake was assessed using an FFQ, and Alzheimer’s disease was assessed by DSM (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders) in both studies (Table 2). The results of these two studies were inconsistent. One study showed that consuming milk less than twice a week was not significantly associated with the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease, compared with daily or 2 to 4 times a week milk intake [37]. Overall, the ROB of this study was rated as being moderate, primarily due to concerns about the representativeness of the study population and inadequate methods used to ascertain exposure (Table 4). The other study reported that higher milk and dairy intake significantly reduced the risk of Alzheimer’s disease (P for trend = 0.03 [38]) (Table 3). Overall, the ROB of this study was rated as being low (Table 4).

Milk and dairy intake and vascular dementia

Two cohort studies assessed the relationship between milk or dairy intake and vascular dementia, assessed by DSM [37, 38]. The characteristics of these two cohorts were described earlier (Table 2). Similar to Alzheimer’s disease outcomes, these two studies showed inconsistent results for vascular dementia outcomes. Specifically, the first study reported that the almost daily milk intake group (vs. < 4 times a week of milk intake group) had a significantly reduced risk of vascular dementia [37], while the second study showed nonsignificant associations between quartiles of milk and dairy intake and risk of vascular dementia [38]. Overall, the ROB of the two studies was rated as being low and moderate, as described earlier (Table 4).

Milk and dairy intake and all-cause dementia

One cohort study assessed the association between milk and dairy intake and all-cause dementia, assessed by DSM III, after 17 years of follow-up [38]. The characteristics of this study were described earlier (Table 2). The results showed no significant associations between quartiles of milk and dairy intake and risk for developing all-cause dementia (Table 3). Overall, the ROB of this study was rated as being low (Table 4).

Overall strength of evidence (SoE)

We graded the overall SoE as being inadequate for the causal relationships between milk consumption and cognitive decline/impairment, all-cause/vascular dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease (Table 5).

Table 5 SoE grading: higher vs. lower milk intake by outcomea

Discussion

This systematic review revealed that the current evidence (7 cohort studies and 1 RCT) is inadequate to draw a conclusion for the causal relationship between milk or dairy intake and cognitive decline or disorders in older adults. The included cohort studies showed large clinical and methodological heterogeneity, hampering the comparability of the study findings. Specifically, milk or dairy intake assessments were heterogeneous, in that studies used various instruments to measure intake (e.g., food frequency questionnaires [FFQ], 24-h recall, or diet history questionnaires) and definitions of milk intake. The types of milk were also variably defined across studies, such as full-cream milk, dairy products, milk and dairy, or fat-reduced dairy foods. Most cohort studies were conducted in generally healthy populations [32,33,34, 36,37,38], except for one [35] that was conducted among individuals with diabetes. Moreover, the included cohort studies utilized a variety of assessment tools to diagnose age-related cognitive disorders and adjusted for different sets of confounding factors in the statistical analyses.

The strength of this systematic review is that we followed the highest methodological standards for evidence synthesis and employed strict inclusion criteria regarding the study designs, which can better determine a causal inference. The previous meta-analysis included cross-sectional studies and did not consider risk-of-bias in their analyses or in drawing their conclusions [21]. Because cross-sectional studies cannot assess the temporality between milk intake and cognitive function, we selected only prospective cohort studies and intervention studies for this systematic review. However, our meta-analysis is limited because of the concerns of risk-of-bias in the included cohort studies (e.g., potential residual confounding) and imprecise measurements of milk intake due to the limitations of FFQs for assessing dietary exposures in observational studies. Additionally, our meta-analysis was based on extreme quantile (highest vs. lowest quintile/quartile) comparisons, which has very limited interpretability, because the definitions of intake levels varied across studies. The variations in exposure levels may explain the large statistical heterogeneity (I2 = 64%; P = 0.06) in the meta-analysis, but we could not conduct a dose-response meta-analysis due to insufficient quantitative data to estimate and standardized the ‘doses’ of milk intake levels.

In light of the limitations in this body of evidence, we have a few suggestions for future research. First, the use of biomarkers would overcome the limitations of self-reported dietary assessments. These self-reported dietary assessments are prone to measurement errors due to recall bias, under/over-reporting, and incompleteness of the food composition database [39, 40]. Because self-reported dietary measures rely heavily on the responders’ recall, recall bias is a particularly important issue in the study of cognitive function. Mild cognitive impairment has shown to attenuate the validity of FFQs when comparing to biomarkers of nutrient intake [41, 42]. Poor cognitive ability was associated with suspected recall errors on the FFQ [43]. As an objective complementary tool for dietary assessment, milk intake biomarkers, such as adipose tissue and circulating levels of pentadecanoic acid (C15:0) [44], would be very useful. Second, there is an urgent need to develop validated, standardized tests of cognitive impairment and dementia to make the research outcomes comparable and able to be quantitatively combined. We found substantial heterogeneity in the assessment tools across the included studies and poor descriptions of outcome measures. Each outcome measure for cognitive impairment and dementia exclusively assessed cognition, function, and other domains (e.g., quality of life, mood, and behavior) or a combination of these domains [45]. To increase the usefulness of SCD measures, the international SCD Initiative Working Group issued a call for international collaboration to promote the harmonization and pooling of cognitive self-reported data and greater consistency in the measurement of cognitive decline [12]. The working group had agreed to a common framework and research procedures to study the role of SCD as a marker of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease [9]. Specifically, there is a need to derive a small number of well-constructed, easy-to-administer items with adequate reliability across diverse samples of older adults. In summary, future research could be improved through the use of milk intake biomarkers and standardized assessment tools for cognitive function to strengthen causal inferences.

Conclusions

Based on best available evidence, we concluded that the overall strength of evidence is inadequate for the effects of milk or dairy consumption on cognitive decline and disorders, due to the insufficient number of high-quality studies and large heterogeneity across studies. To draw a firm conclusion, high quality RCTs with sufficient sample sizes, use of milk intake biomarkers, and standardized assessment tools for cognitive function are needed to identify the role of milk or dairy intake in cognitive function among older adults.

Abbreviations

DECO:

‘DEtérioration Cognitive Observée’ (observed cognitive deterioration)

DSM:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

DSST:

Digital Symbol Substitution Test

DWRT:

Delayed Word Recall Test

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

GDS:

Global Deterioration Scale

IADL:

Instrumental Activities of Daily Living

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

MFGM:

Milk fat globule membrane

MMSE:

Mini Mental Status Examination

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

NIA-AA:

National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroup

NOS:

Newcastle Ottawa Scale

PRISMA:

Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses

RCT:

Randomized controlled trial

ROB:

Risk of bias

SCD:

Subjective Cognitive Decline

SoE:

Strength of evidence

WFT:

Word Fluency Test

References

  1. United Nations. World population ageing. New York: UN; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, Ganguli M, Hall K, Hasegawa K, Hendrie H, Huang Y. Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet. 2006;366:2112–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Srivastava G, Tripathi RK, Tiwari SC, Singh B, Tripathi SM. Caregiver burden and quality of life of key caregivers of patients with dementia. Indian J Psychol Med. 2016;38:133–6.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Crichton GE, Bryan J, Murphy KJ, Buckley J. Review of dairy consumption and cognitive performance in adults: findings and methodological issues. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:352–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Balzac F. Subjective cognitive decline: the earliest sign of alzheimer’s disease? Neurology Rev. 2013;21:33–7.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Cheng YW, Chen TF, Chiu MJ. From mild cognitive impairment to subjective cognitive decline: conceptual and methodological evolution. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2017;13:491–8.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Mitchell AJ. Is it time to separate subjective cognitive complaints from the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment? Age Ageing. 2008;37:497–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, Breteler M, Ceccaldi M, Chetelat G, Dubois B, Dufouil C, Ellis KA, van der Flier WM, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10:844–52.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, Iwatsubo T, Jack CR Jr, Kaye J, Montine TJ, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:280–92.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, Buckley R, Dubois B, Ellis KA, Ewers M, Hampel H, Kloppel S, Rami L, et al. Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13:296–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Crane PK, Amariglio RE, Berman LM, Boada M, Buckley RF, Chetelat G, Dubois B, Ellis KA, et al. Subjective cognitive decline in older adults: an overview of self-report measures used across 19 international research studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;48(Suppl 1):S63–86.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Studart AN, Nitrini R. Subjective cognitive decline: the first clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease? Dement Neuropsychol. 2016;10:170–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Vega JN, Newhouse PA. Mild cognitive impairment: diagnosis, longitudinal course, and emerging treatments. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2014;16:490.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med. 2014;275:214–28.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Humpel C. Identifying and validating biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. Trends Biotechnol. 2011;29:26–32.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Hess JM, Jonnalagadda SS, Slavin JL. Dairy foods: current evidence of their effects on bone, cardiometabolic, cognitive, and digestive health. Compr Rev Food Sci Food Saf. 2016;15:251–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Spitsberg V. Invited review: bovine milk fat globule membrane as a potential nutraceutical. J Dairy Sci. 2005;88:2289–94.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Verardo V, Gómez-Caravaca AM, Arráez-Román D, Hettinga K. Recent advances in phospholipids from colostrum, milk and dairy by-products. Int J Mol Sci. 2017;18:173.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Conway V, Gauthier SF, Pouliot Y. Buttermilk: much more than a source of milk phospholipids. Anim Front. 2014;4:44–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Wu L, Sun D. Meta-analysis of milk consumption and the risk of cognitive disorders. Nutrients. 2016;8:824.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, and the PG: Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the prisma statement. Ann Intern Med 2009, 151:264–269.

  23. Wallace BC, Small K, Brodley CE, Lau J, Trikalinos TA. Deploying an interactive machine learning system in an evidence-based practice center: abstrackr. In: Proceedings of the 2nd ACM SIGHIT International Health Informatics Symposium. Boston: ACM; 2012. p. 819–24.

  24. Wells GA, Shea B, O'Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, Tugwell P. The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Ottawa: Ottawa Hospital Research Institute; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Higgins J, Sterne J, Savović J, Page M, Hróbjartsson A, Boutron I, Reeves B, Eldridge S. A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomized trials. Cochrane Methods. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016; Issue 10(Suppl 1).

  26. Cho SS, Qi L, Fahey GC, Klurfeld DM. Consumption of cereal fiber, mixtures of whole grains and bran, and whole grains and risk reduction in type 2 diabetes, obesity, and cardiovascular disease. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98:594–619.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Introduction. The American Diabetes Association’s (ADA) evidence-based practice guidelines, standards, and related recommendations and documents for diabetes care. Diabetes Care. 2012;35:S1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7:177–88.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Higgins JP, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327:557–60.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Crichton GE, Murphy KJ, Howe PR, Buckley JD, Bryan J. Dairy consumption and working memory performance in overweight and obese adults. Appetite. 2012;59:34–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Kesse-Guyot E, Assmann KE, Andreeva VA, Ferry M, Hercberg S, Galan P. Consumption of dairy products and cognitive functioning: findings from the SU.VI.MAX 2 study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2016;20:128–37.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Petruski-Ivleva N, Kucharska-Newton A, Palta P, Couper D, Meyer K, Graff M, Haring B, Sharrett R, Heiss G. Milk intake at midlife and cognitive decline over 20 years. the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) Study. Nutrients. 2017;9:1134.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Vercambre M-N, Boutron-Ruault M-C, Ritchie K, Clavel-Chapelon F, Berr C. Long-term association of food and nutrient intakes with cognitive and functional decline: a 13-year follow-up study of elderly French women. Br J Nutr. 2009;102:419–27.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Araki A, Yoshimura Y, Sakurai T, Umegaki H, Kamada C, Iimuro S, Ohashi Y, Ito H. Low intakes of carotene, vitamin B2, pantothenate and calcium predict cognitive decline among elderly patients with diabetes mellitus: the Japanese elderly diabetes intervention trial. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2017;17:1168–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Almeida OP, Norman P, Hankey G, Jamrozik K, Flicker L. Successful mental health aging: results from a longitudinal study of older Australian men. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:27–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Yamada M, Kasagi F, Sasaki H, Masunari N, Mimori Y, Suzuki G. Association between dementia and midlife risk factors: the radiation effects research foundation adult health study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:410–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Ozawa M, Ohara T, Ninomiya T, Hata J, Yoshida D, Mukai N, Nagata M, Uchida K, Shirota T, Kitazono T. Milk and dairy consumption and risk of dementia in an elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62:1224–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Jenab M, Slimani N, Bictash M, Ferrari P, Bingham SA. Biomarkers in nutritional epidemiology: applications, needs and new horizons. Hum Genet. 2009;125:507–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Baylin A, Campos H. The use of fatty acid biomarkers to reflect dietary intake. Curr Opin Lipidol. 2006;17:22–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Bowman GL, Shannon J, Ho E, Traber MG, Frei B, Oken BS, Kaye JA, Quinn JF. Reliability and validity of food frequency questionnaire and nutrient biomarkers in elders with and without mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2011;25:49–57.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Zuniga K, McAuley E. Considerations in selection of diet assessment methods for examining the effect of nutrition on cognition. J Nutr Health Aging. 2015;19:333–40.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Pope SK, Kritchevsky SB, Morris MC, Block G, Tylavsky FA, Lee JS, Stewart S, Harris T, Rubin SM, Simonsick EM. Cognitive ability is associated with suspected reporting errors on food frequency questionnaires. J Nutr Health Aging. 2007;11:55–8.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Riserus U, Marklund M. Milk fat biomarkers and cardiometabolic disease. Curr Opin Lipidol. 2017;28:46–51.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Harrison JK, Noel-Storr AH, Demeyere N, Reynish EL, Quinn TJ. Outcomes measures in a decade of dementia and mild cognitive impairment trials. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2016;8:48.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Jian Shi for performing the abstract and full-text screening, data extraction, and risk-of-bias assessment for this project.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Research Council of Science & Technology (NST) (No. CAP-16-07-KIOM, GN160800–01) and Korea Food Research Institute (E0150302–03), South Korea.

Availability of data and materials

We included 5 tables and 2 figures in this manuscript. Additionally, there are 3 supplementary tables and a supplemental data file online.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HL, JL, DJ, and MC designed research; ZF and JL conducted research; ZF and MC analyzed data; and JL, ZF, and MC interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hae-Jeung Lee.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Table S1. Search strategy. Table S2. Detailed instructions of the modified NOS tool. Table S3. SoE grading system. (DOC 83 kb)

Additional file 2:

Analytical dataset. (XLSX 9 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lee, J., Fu, Z., Chung, M. et al. Role of milk and dairy intake in cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr J 17, 82 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-018-0387-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-018-0387-1

Keywords