Skip to main content
  • Letter to the Editor
  • Open access
  • Published:

Response to important considerations when assessing the effect of essential fatty acids on cognitive performance

A Letter to the Editor to this article was published on 11 September 2020

Abstract

In this letter, we respond to the comments raised by Visaria et al. in their letter to the editor about the publication “Association of dietary ω-3 and ω-6 fatty acids intake with cognitive performance in older adults: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011–2014”. We have further adjusted for some key covariates as the authors mentioned in the letter and performed sensitivity analysis by excluding vegetarians considering the bioavailability of fatty acids from different sources. In conclusion, the results were basically consistent with our previous results, which showed that the results were stable and reliable. We hope that our study could be helpful in further studies delineating the various intricacies of fatty acid nutrition and metabolism and control for covariates.

Peer Review reports

To the Editor:

We would like to thank you for the opportunity to respond to the comments raised by Visaria et al. in their letter to the editor about the publication “Association of dietary ω-3 and ω-6 fatty acids intake with cognitive performance in older adults: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011–2014”. We would also like to thank Visaria et al. for their interest in our paper and for taking the time to express their views. We will answer the questions in detail about covariates and methodological considerations raised by the authors.

Visaria and colleagues’ first comment pertains to the control of covariates. They contend that while we have appropriately controlled for demographic, socioeconomic, physical activity, and cardiometabolic comorbidity factors, there are several other covariates that need to be accounted for, such as vitamin, mineral and other dietary consumption, sociobehavioral risk factors and physical & mental comorbidities. According to their suggestion and our sample size (2496 participants), we further adjusted for vitamin D [1], niacin [2], vitamin B6 [3], vitamin B12, folic acid, zinc [4], iron, copper, selenium, protein [5], total saturated fatty acids [6], and depression [7] in Model 2. Table 1 presents the characteristics of the study population across cognitive status. There were no significant differences between people with low cognitive performance and normal cognitive performance in the distribution of pre-diabetes, gastrointestinal disorders, and smoking among three tests. So, we did not put them into the overall analysis. Table 2 shows the associations of ω-3 fatty acids, ω-6 fatty acids, and ω-6: ω-3 ratio with three tests. In the full-adjusted model, the odds ratios (ORs) with 95% confidence interval (CI) of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) test score, Animal Fluency test score and the Digit Symbol Substitution test (DSST) score were 0.59(0.38–0.92), 0.69(0.46–1.06) and 0.57(0.39–0.83) for the highest versus lowest tertile of ω-3 fatty acids, respectively; the ORs with 95% CI of CERAD test score, Animal Fluency test score and DSST score were 0.50(0.32–0.79), 0.64(0.42–0.98) and 0.55(0.37–0.81) for the highest versus lowest tertile of ω-6 fatty acids, respectively. The association between ω-6: ω-3 ratio and cognitive performance was not statistically significant in three tests. Although the association between ω-3 fatty acids and Animal Fluency test did not reach statistical significance, the OR of Animal Fluency test was 0.69. Overall, the above results were basically consistent with our previous results [8], which showed that the results were stable and reliable.

Table 1 Characteristics of the study population, NHANES 2011–2014 (N = 2496)
Table 2 Weighted odds ratios (95% confidence intervals) for score on CERAD test, Animal Fluency test, and DSST across tertiles of dietary ω-3 and ω-6 fatty acids intake and ω-6: ω-3 ratio, NHANES 2011–2014 (N = 2496)

The second comment outlined by Visaria et al. in their letter to the editor is the type and source of fatty acid (plant-based vs. animal-based), in context of an individual’s overall dietary patterns. As the authors mentioned, the source of the fatty acid can impact how it is metabolized in the body and its resulting bioavailability. We agree with the authors’ point and we divided participants into two groups (2146 non-vegetarians and 350 vegetarians). However, the question “Do you consider yourself to be a vegetarian?” was only asked in NHANES 2007–2010 [9], so we used two 24-h dietary recall to assess the types of foods the participants ate. Furthermore, considering that vegetarians made up only 14% of the participants, we performed sensitivity analysis by excluding vegetarians in Table 3. In the fully adjusted model, the negative associations of CERAD test and DSST with ω-3 fatty acids and ω-6 fatty acids were still significant. The association of Animal Fluency test with ω-3 fatty acids and ω-6 fatty acids were not statistically significant.

Table 3 Weighted odds ratios (95% confidence intervals) for score on CERAD test, Animal Fluency test, and DSST across tertiles of dietary ω-3 and ω-6 fatty acids intake and ω-6: ω-3 ratio excluding vegetariansb, NHANES 2011–2014 (N = 2146)

Regarding the comment from Visaria et al. that variability of 24-h dietary recall from first interview to second interview need to be considered to get an idea of an individual’s dietary consistency, we performed correlation test between the two recalls. We found that the correlation between the two recalls was significant (correlation coefficient: 0.260 for ω-3 fatty acids, 0.295 for ω-6 fatty acids), so we used a combination of the first-day and second-day mean values to make use of all available dietary data. Furthermore, some studies have shown that two 24-h recalls might be sufficient to assess the daily dietary intake [10]. In addition, this self-recall might be affected by one’s cognitive function and lead to potential bias as the authors noted, we agree with the authors and will discuss the limitation for the method in further studies.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are available in publicly repository as described below. The authors do not own the data. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey data are available from the National Center for Health Statistics (http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/ nhanes_questionnaires.htm).

Abbreviations

NHANES:

The National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys

CERAD:

The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s disease

DSST:

The Digit Symbol Substitution Test

OR:

Odds ratio

CI:

Confidence interval

References

  1. Goodwill AM, Szoeke C. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of low vitamin D on cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65:2161–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.15012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Morris MC, Evans DA, Bienias JL, Scherr PA, Tangney CC, Hebert LE, et al. Dietary niacin and the risk of incident Alzheimer's disease and of cognitive decline. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:1093–9. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2003.025858.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Moore K, Hughes CF, Ward M, Hoey L, McNulty H. Diet, nutrition and the ageing brain: current evidence and new directions. Proc Nutr Soc. 2018;77:152–63. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0029665117004177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Li S, Sun W, Zhang D. Association of zinc, iron, copper, and selenium intakes with low cognitive performance in older adults: a cross-sectional study from National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;72:1145–57. https://doi.org/10.3233/jad-190263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Li Y, Li S, Wang W, Zhang D. Association between dietary protein intake and cognitive function in adults aged 60 years and older. J Nutr Health Aging. 2020;24:223–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-020-1317-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Ortega RM, Requejo AM, Andrés P, López-Sobaler AM, Quintas ME, Redondo MR, et al. Dietary intake and cognitive function in a group of elderly people. Am J Clin Nutr. 1997;66:803–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/66.4.803.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Zhang R, Sun J, Li Y, Zhang D. Associations of n−3, n-6 fatty acids intakes and n-6:n-3 ratio with the risk of depressive symptoms: NHANES 2009-2016. Nutrients. 2020:12. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12010240.

  8. Dong X, Li S, Chen J, Li Y, Wu Y, Zhang D. Association of dietary ω-3 and ω-6 fatty acids intake with cognitive performance in older adults: National Health and nutrition examination Survey (NHANES) 2011-2014. Nutr J. 2020;19:25. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-020-00547-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2007-2008, Data Documentation, Diet Behavior & Nutrition. https://wwwn.cdc.gov/Nchs/Nhanes/2007-2008/DBQ_E.htm Accessed 31 May 2020.

  10. Knuppel S, Norman K, Boeing H. Is a single 24-hour dietary recall per person sufficient to estimate the population distribution of usual dietary intake? J Nutr. 2019;149:1491–2. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/nxz118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the staff at the National Center for Health Statistics at the CDC, who design, collect, administer the NHANES data and release the data available for public use. We are thankful to all study participants for their cooperation.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

XD and DZ conceived and designed the study; XD wrote the manuscript. All authors edited the manuscript and provided their ideas. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dongfeng Zhang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dong, X., Zhang, D. Response to important considerations when assessing the effect of essential fatty acids on cognitive performance. Nutr J 19, 99 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-020-00620-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-020-00620-1

Keywords