Skip to main content

Association between frequency of breakfast intake before and during pregnancy and developmental delays in children: the Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study

Abstract

Background

Although an association between maternal nutritional intake and developmental delays in children has been demonstrated, the association of the timing of meal intake and development delays remains unclear. We examined the association between breakfast intake frequency before and during pregnancy and developmental delay in children.

Methods

Of the pregnant women who participated in the Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Three-Generation Cohort Study, 7491 answered the required questions and were analyzed. The frequency of breakfast intake from pre- to early pregnancy and from early to mid-pregnancy was classified into four groups: daily, and 5–6, 3–4, and 0–2 times/week. Child developmental delays at age 2 and 3.5 years were assessed using the Ages & Stages Questionnaire, Third Edition. Logistic regression models were constructed to examine the association between breakfast intake frequency in pregnant women and developmental delays in children aged 2 and 3.5 years.

Results

The proportion of pregnant women who had breakfast daily was 78.1% in pre- to early pregnancy, and 82.2% in early to mid-pregnancy. The proportion of children with developmental delays was 14.7% and 13.4% at age 2 and 3.5 years, respectively. Compared with the risk in children of women who had breakfast daily from pre- to early pregnancy, children of women who had breakfast 0–2 times/week had a higher risk of developmental delays at 2 years of age: odds ratio (OR) 1.30, (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.02–1.66). The risk of developmental delays at age 2 years increased in the children of women who had breakfast 0–2 times/week in early to mid- pregnancy: OR 1.75 (95% CI, 1.32–2.32). The risk of developmental delays at age 3.5 years did not increase in the children of women who had breakfast 0–2 times/week from pre- to early and early to mid-pregnancy: OR 1.06 (95% CI, 0.81–1.39 and OR 1.15 (95% CI 0.84–1.57), respectively.

Conclusion

For women with a low frequency of breakfast intake from pre- to mid-pregnancy there was an association with developmental delays in their children at age 2, but not at 3.5 years.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Developmental delay is the failure to meet developmental milestones compared to peers [1]. Delays are observed in speech, language, and cognitive development, and also in the ability to adapt to society [2]. Children’s developmental delays result in public health problems with a range of negative consequences, including academic and career difficulties [3], suicide [4] and premature death [5]. The number of children with developmental delays is increasing [6], affecting approximately 10% of children worldwide [7]. Genetic and environmental factors during fetal life may contribute to developmental delays in children [8, 9].

Studies have demonstrated associations between nutrients or foods consumed by pregnant women and developmental delays in the child, including folic acid and multivitamin supplements [10], vegetables, fruit [11], and fish [12], which have been shown to be associated with a reduced risk of developmental delays in the child. The association between dietary patterns such as the Mediterranean and Western diets and developmental delays in children remains unclear [13].

Irregular lifestyle habits of mothers may be associated with developmental delays in their children. Skipping breakfast is a component of irregular lifestyle habits [14], and regular breakfast intake has been associated with the formation of an accurate circadian rhythm [15] and a lower risk of lifestyle-related diseases [16]. Studies have also demonstrated that skipping breakfast during pregnancy is associated with an increased risk of poor diet quality [17], gestational diabetes [18], hypertensive disorders of pregnancy [19], and reduced birth weight [20]. We hypothesized that skipping breakfast would affect nutrient intake and circadian rhythms, and could be associated with developmental delays in children. However, to the best of our knowledge, no studies have examined the association between maternal skipping of breakfast and developmental delays in their children.

In the present study, we examined the association between the frequency of breakfast intake from pre- to mid-pregnancy and developmental delays in children aged 2 and 3.5 years of age. In addition, we investigated the association between changes in the frequency of breakfast intake from pre- to mid-pregnancy and developmental delays in children to evaluate the appropriate period required for breakfast intake.

Materials and methods

Study design and population

Data were obtained from the Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study (TMM BirThree Cohort Study), the details of which have been described elsewhere [21]. Pregnant women and their families were recruited between 2013 and 2017 from obstetrics and gynecology clinics or hospitals where deliveries were scheduled. Approximately 50 obstetrics and gynecology clinics and hospitals in Miyagi Prefecture participated in the recruitment process. In total, 23,406 mothers and children were enrolled in this study. Among them, 14,593 mother–child pairs were excluded because of incomplete questionnaires, lack of permission to transcribe medical records, birth defects, or type 1 or type 2 diabetes. Of the remaining 8813 mother–child pairs, 1322 were excluded because of missing values for the analytic variables. The remaining 7491 mother–child pairs were included in this study (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of participant inclusion. aFFQ: Food Frequency Questionnaire. bASQ: Ages and Stages questionnaire. cBMI: body mass index

Exposure variables

The frequency of breakfast and other food intake was assessed using a 130-item self-administered semiquantitative food frequency questionnaire (FFQ). The FFQs used in this study were made by modifying the FFQ used in the Japan Public Health Center-based Prospective study [22, 23] and were added to the response option “constitutionally unable to eat it” to identify genetic factors. The first FFQ assessed the frequency and quantity of foods and beverages consumed in the preceding year, while the second FFQ assessed those consumed since the administration of the first FFQ. The mean response periods for the first and second FFQ completed by the participants and included in the analysis were 19.3 ± 7·0 weeks and 27·5 ± 5·3 weeks of gestation, respectively. Breakfast intake frequency was assessed using the question, “How often do you eat breakfast?”. The categories of intake frequency in the response items were as follows: less than once a month, 1–3 times a month, 1–2 times a week, 3–4 times a week, 5–6 times a week, and daily. The first three categories were combined into 0–2 times per week because of the small sample size. To examine the association between changes in the frequency of breakfast intake from pre- to mid-pregnancy and developmental delays in the child, we defined those who had breakfast daily or 5–6 times a week as “With breakfast intake” and those who had breakfast 3–4 times a week or 0–2 times a week as “Without breakfast intake [17]”. The four categories were defined as follows: women whose response to the first and second FFQ was “With breakfast intake” (With–With); women whose response to the first FFQ was “With breakfast intake” and whose response to the second FFQ was “Without breakfast intake” (With–Without); women whose response to the first FFQ was “Without breakfast intake” and whose response to the second FFQ was “With breakfast intake” (Without–With); and women whose response to the first and second FFQ was “Without breakfast intake” (Without–Without).

Outcome variables

The Japanese version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ-3), 3rd Edition was used to assess the child’s development [24, 25]. The ASQ-3 is a comprehensive and reliable screening questionnaire that can be used for children aged 1–66 months. Each questionnaire contained 30 questions divided into five developmental domains: “communication”, “fine motor”, “gross motor”, “problem-solving”, and “personal-social”. Each domain has a set of six items, and each item is scored as 10, 5, or 0, corresponding to’ yes, sometimes, or not yet, respectively. The total score ranged from 0 to 60 for each domain. More than two standard deviations below the mean were the referral cut-offs for each developmental domain, and children were identified as having developmental delays if their scores fell below the referral cut-off in any of the five domains [26, 27].

Covariates

Based on previous studies [11,12,13, 18,19,20], the following covariates were selected: age at delivery, pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI), educational level, smoking status, alcohol consumption, parity, employment status, folic acid supplement intake, morning sickness, insomnia, psychological distress, postnatal bonding disorder, food intake (cereals, meat, seafood, legumes, vegetables, and fruits), child's sex, and breastfeeding at 1 year. Age at delivery, height, pre-pregnancy weight, and parity were ascertained from participants’ medical records. Age at delivery was categorized as < 24, 25–29, 30–34, and ≥ 35 years. Pre-pregnancy BMI was calculated by dividing the pre-pregnancy weight by the square of height, and was divided into three categories: underweight (< 18.5 kg/m2), normal (18.5–24.9 kg/m2), and overweight/obesity (≥ 25.0 kg/m2). Parity was dichotomized into none and more than one. Data on educational level were obtained one year after birth and classified into three categories: high school or lower, college or special training school, and university or higher. Variables related to smoking status, alcohol consumption, folic acid supplementation during early pregnancy, morning sickness, employment status, psychological distress, and insomnia were obtained using a questionnaire administered during early pregnancy. The woman’s smoking status was divided into four categories: never, quit before pregnancy, quit after pregnancy, and current. Alcohol consumption was divided into three categories: never, former, and current. Folic acid supplement intake was dichotomized as yes or no. Morning sickness was classified into four categories: never, former, and current. Employment status was classified into three categories: employed, not employed, and other. Psychological distress was assessed using the Japanese version of the K6 [28, 29], with psychological distress defined as a score of ≥ 5 [29, 30]. Insomnia was assessed using the Japanese version of the Athens Insomnia Scale, [31] and was defined as a score of ≥ 6 [31, 32]. Postnatal bonding disorder was assessed using a questionnaire at one month postpartum and based on the Japanese version of the Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale [33]. Postnatal bonding disorder was defined as a score on the Japanese version of the Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale of ≥ 5 [34]. The intake of each food was calculated as the average intake based on the frequency of intake of cereals, meat, seafood, pulses, vegetables, and fruits from pre-pregnancy to early pregnancy, and early to mid-pregnancy, and was adjusted for energy using the residual method [35]. First, total energy and nutrient intakes were calculated using data from the Standard Tables of Food Composition in Japan (Fifth Revised and Enlarged Edition 2005) [36]. Data on the sex of the children were obtained from medical records. As birth weight is a post-exposure covariate and considered a mediator rather than a covariate, we did not adjust for this covariate. Breastfeeding at 1 year of age was determined using data from the child's 1 year old questionnaire.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using analysis of variance for continuous variables and the chi-square test for categorical variables, to examine the differences in the characteristics of pregnant women according to breakfast intake frequency. Logistic regression analyses were performed to determine the associations between the frequency of breakfast consumption and developmental delays for each of the five domains of the ASQ-3 at 2 and 3.5 years of age. The odds ratio (OR), 95% confidence interval (CI), and P for trends were calculated and adjusted for covariates. The association between changes in the frequency of breakfast intake and developmental delays in children was assessed in the same way as that used to calculate the ORs and 95% CIs. In addition, because stress during pregnancy is a risk factor for developmental delays [37], sensitivity analyses stratified by insomnia, psychological distress, and employment status were performed.

All the statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Statistical significance was set at a p-value < 0.05.

Results

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the pregnant women and children according to their breakfast intake frequency. The mean maternal age at the child’s birth was 31.9 ± 4.6 years. For women who had breakfast daily, the frequency was 78.1% and 82.2% in the pre- to early and early to mid-pregnancy periods, respectively. Compared to women who had breakfast daily, those who skipped breakfast more frequently were younger at delivery, had higher rates of overweight/obesity, and psychological distress. They reported a lower education, that it was their first childbirth, and that they experienced insomnia. Women who skipped breakfast more frequently also had lower energy, cereal, and vegetable intakes. The percentage of children with developmental delays was 14.7% at 2 years of age and 13.4% at 3.5 years of age.

Table 1 Characteristics of the participants

The characteristics of the 7491 mother–child pairs that were analyzed, and 15,915 excluded mother–child pairs are shown in Supplementary Table 1. Compared to the study sample overall, women in the analysis sample had a higher age at delivery, a higher level of education and a lower rate of developmental delay in their children. The TMM BirThree Cohort Study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tohoku University Tohoku Medical Megabank Organization (2013–1-103–1).

Table 2 shows the association between the frequency of breakfast intake and developmental delays in children at 2 years of age. Compared to the values associated with children of women who had breakfast daily from pre- to early pregnancy, the adjusted ORs (95% CIs), of developmental delays in children for women who had breakfast 5–6, 3–4, and 0–2 times/week were 0.95 (0.75–1.21), 1.09 (0.83–1.44), and 1.30 (1.02–1.66), respectively. Compared to the values associated with children of women who had breakfast daily from early to mid-pregnancy, the adjusted ORs (95% CIs), of developmental delays in children for women who had breakfast 5–6, 3–4, and 0–2 times/week were 0.98 (0.77–1.25), 1.22 (0.90–1.65), and 1.75 (1.32–2.32), respectively. The association between the frequency of breakfast intake during pregnancy and each of the five ASQ-3 domains at 2 and 3.5 years of age is shown in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3. The communication domain of children of women who had breakfast less than four times/week before and during pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of delays at the age of 2 years, but in none of the developmental domains at age 3.5 years.

Table 2 Association between frequency of breakfast intake during pregnancy and children’s developmental delays at 2 years

Table 3 shows the association between breakfast intake frequency and developmental delays at 3.5 years of age. Compared to the values associated with children of women who had breakfast daily from pre- to early pregnancy, the adjusted ORs (95% CIs), of developmental delays in children for women who had breakfast 5–6 times/week, 3–4 times/week, and 0–2 times/week were 1.13 (0.89–1.43), 0.99 (0.74–1.34), and 1.06 (0.81–1.39), respectively. Compared to the values associated with children of women who had breakfast daily from early to mid-pregnancy, the adjusted ORs (95% CIs), of developmental delays in children for women who had breakfast 5–6, 3–4, and 0–2 times/week were 0.95 (0.74–1.23), 1.04 (0.76–1.44), and 1.15 (0.84–1.57), respectively.

Table 3 Association between frequency of breakfast intake during pregnancy and children's developmental delays at 3.5 years

Table 4 shows the association between changes in breakfast intake frequency and developmental delays in children. Compared to the values associated with children of women who had breakfast daily before and during pregnancy (With–With), the OR (95% CI), for developmental delays at 2 years of age in children of women who only had breakfast pre- to early pregnancy (With–Without) was 1.60 (1.22–2.10). The OR (95% CI), for developmental delays at 2 years of age for children of women who only had breakfast early to mid-pregnancy (Without–With) was 1.06 (0.78–1.43). The OR (95% CI), for developmental delays at 2 years of age in the children of women who did not have breakfast before and during (Without–Without) was 1.16 (0.69–1.97). In a similar comparison with developmental delays at 2 years, the adjusted ORs (95% CIs) of developmental delays at 3.5 years for children were 1.19 (0.89–1.60), 1.01 (0.74–1.40), and 0.62 (0.32–1.21), respectively. Factors in the With–Without group that were associated with children's development at 2 years of age were higher rates of smoking, severe morning sickness, insomnia, psychological distress, and low birth weights.

Table 4 Association between frequency changes of breakfast intake from pre- to mid-pregnancy and children’s developmental delays

Sensitivity analyses stratified by psychological distress, insomnia and employment status showed that the association between breakfast intake frequency and developmental delay in the child was similar to the main results (Supplementary Table 4).

Discussion

This study showed that less frequent breakfast intake during pre- to mid-pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of developmental delays in children at 2 years of age, but not with developmental delays at 3.5 years of age. To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate an association between breakfast intake frequency in pregnant women and developmental delays in children. In addition, compared to the children of pregnant women who had breakfast during pre- to mid-pregnancy, the children of pregnant women who had breakfast during pre- to early pregnancy, but not during early to mid-pregnancy, had a higher risk of developmental delays at 2 years of age. In contrast, there was no association with developmental delay in children aged 3.5 years of age.

There are several possible reasons why the present study found an association between breakfast frequency in pregnant women and development at 2 years of age. Although different cultures consume different foods at breakfast, a typical Japanese breakfast is rich in soy products, fish, seaweed, vegetables, fruit and green tea [38]. In the present study, mothers who frequently skipped breakfast had a lower intake of energy, cereals, as well as the fish, seaweed and vegetables consumed in a typical Japanese breakfast. However, the association between less frequent breakfast intake in pregnant women and developmental delays in children at 2 years of age remained even after adjusting for these dietary covariates. This result suggests that the mother's irregular lifestyle may influence the child's developmental delays.

At approximately 20 weeks of gestation, the fetus begins to develop an internal clock that is modelled on the mother's circadian rhythm [39]. The regulatory part of the body clock in the brain begins to function at approximately 30 weeks of gestation and matures at approximately 37 weeks of gestation [40]. If the basis of the body clock is not properly established during the fetal period, the supernormal rhythms underlying the body clock will not be properly established [41]. Irregular mealtimes and extreme late nights during pregnancy have been reported to cause disruption of the mother's “biological clock”, which may adversely affect the child's development [42]. A study of pregnant women in Japan showed that low physical activity levels and too little or too much sleep was associated with delayed neurodevelopment of the child [43]. Another study showed that a habit of sleeping past midnight every day and irregular mealtimes were associated with irritable sleep–wake rhythm abnormalities [44]. Thus, irregular mealtimes are detrimental to the physical and mental health of mothers and children, and therefore should be avoided. This study showed that changes in the frequency of breakfast consumption –with breakfast before to early pregnancy and without breakfast in early to mid-pregnancy–were associated with the risk of developmental delays in children aged 2 years. Considering that the biological clock of the fetus begins to form in mid-pregnancy, it seems reasonable that skipping breakfast only during mid-pregnancy affects the formation of the fetal body clock. However, the group that skipped breakfast only in mid-pregnancy was characterized by higher rates of smoking and psychological distress. Therefore, the frequency of breakfast intake is one of the possible pathways and may not fully explain the present association.

Eating breakfast less frequency in pre- to mid-pregnancy was only associated with developmental delays in the communication area in children aged 2 years. In a previous study, pregnant women who had low protein intake in pre- to early-pregnancy and were more likely to skip breakfast had children who were associated with developmental delays of communication, fine motor, and problem-solving skills at 3-years of age [45]. Amino acids, the building blocks of proteins, also act as essential precursors for the synthesis of various molecules such as hormones and neurotransmitters, which may influence neurodevelopment [46]. In this study, pregnant women who ate breakfast less often also had a lower protein intake; however, it is unclear why this was only associated with delayed development in the area of communication.

In the present study, the frequency of breakfast consumption among pregnant women was not associated with developmental delays in children aged 3.5 years. Compared with women whose children had developmental delays at 2 and 3.5 years of age, women whose children had developmental delays at 2 years of age and no developmental delays at 3.5 years of age were more likely to be highly educated, employed, and less likely to have insomnia and stress (Supplementary Table 5). Parents who are better educated and more financially comfortable are likely to invest more in their children’s development [47, 48]. Infant health check-ups are conducted at 18 months of age in Japan, and well-educated parents whose children are assessed as developmentally delayed may have invested more in their children’s development to make up for any developmental delay. Because the period from 0 to 3 years of age is critical for the formation and subsequent maintenance of synapses, inappropriate stimulation during this period can have a significant and lasting negative impact on later development [49]. In addition, interventions between the ages of zero and three years can permanently alter children’s developmental trajectories and protect them from lifestyle-related diseases and depression [50]. In the present study, the frequency with which pregnant women ate breakfast was not associated with developmental delay at the age of 3.5 years. However, increasing the frequency of breakfast consumption in pregnant women to prevent developmental delays at 0–3 years of age may have lifelong benefits for the child.

Implication

Our findings have implications for the design of effective interventions aimed at preventing developmental delays in children. If the observed association between a low frequency of breakfast intake among pregnant women and child developmental delays is causal, interventions that promote breakfast intake during pregnancy may help prevent child developmental delays. In Japan, the Dietary Guidelines for Pregnant Women have been revised to emphasize the importance of improving dietary habits before pregnancy [51]. Despite the high rate of skipping breakfast among young Japanese women, Dietary Guidelines do not specify breakfast intake. A better understanding of the association between the frequency of breakfast intake before and during pregnancy and child development will help identify at-risk mother–child pairs and implement effective interventions.

Limitations

This study had several limitations. First, it was conducted in only one of the 47 prefectures in Japan, which limits the generalizability of its findings. Second, the women in this study were older, more educated, and consumed breakfast more frequently than the excluded women did. In addition, the children of the women included in this study had lower rates of developmental delay than those of the children of the excluded women, which could have resulted in a selection bias. Third, data on the frequency of breakfast intake were obtained from participants’ self-reported questionnaires, which could have led to misclassification. Fourth, the ASQ-3 is a screening tool and not a diagnostic tool for developmental delays. However, the reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the ASQ-3 has been confirmed [25]. Fifth, dietary factors other than selected foods may not be adequately adjusted. Although the foods to be adjusted were selected from previous studies [11,12,13, 18,19,20], the possibility that other foods may have influenced the association between maternal breakfast frequency and child developmental delay cannot be ruled out. Sixth, despite careful control for known risk factors and unknown potential confounders, we may not have been able to eliminate the effects of residual confounding, which is inevitable in observational studies. However, in the sensitivity analysis stratified by insomnia, psychological distress, and employment status, the same conclusions were reached as in the main analysis.

Conclusion

A low frequency of breakfast intake before and during pregnancy was associated with developmental delays in the offspring at 2 years of age but not at 3.5 years. Although the mechanisms of this association are not yet fully understood, these findings may provide clues for designing dietary interventions for pregnant women to prevent developmental delays in their children and improve maternal and child health.

Availability of data and materials

The TMM BirThree Cohort Study data that support the findings of this study are not publicly available due to the data containing information that could compromise research participant consent. All inquiries about access to the data should be sent to the TMM.

Abbreviations

ASQ:

Ages and Stages questionnaire

BMI:

Body mass index

CI:

Confidence interval

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

OR:

Odds ratio

TMM:

Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study (TMM BirThree Cohort Study)

References

  1. Choo YY, Agarwal P, How CH, Yeleswarapu SP. Developmental delay: identification and management at primary care level. Singapore Med J. 2019;60(3):119–23. https://doi.org/10.11622/smedj.2019025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Bélanger SA, Caron J. Evaluation of the child with global developmental delay and intellectual disability. Paediatr Child Health. 2018;23(6):403–19. https://doi.org/10.1093/pch/pxy093.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Lu Y, Sjölander A, Cederlöf M, D’Onofrio BM, Almqvist C, Larsson H, et al. Association between medication use and performance on higher education entrance tests in individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiat. 2017;74:815–22. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Hedley D, Uljarević M, Foley KR, Richdale A, Trollor J. Risk and protective factors underlying depression and suicidal ideation in autism spectrum disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2018;35:648–57. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sun SH, Kuja-Halkola R, Faraone SV, D’Onofrio BM, Dalsgaard S, Chang Z, et al. Association of psychiatric comorbidity with the risk of premature death among children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiat. 2019;76:1141–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Thapar A, Cooper M, Rutter M. Neurodevelopmental disorders. Lancet. Psychiatry. 2017;4:339–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30376-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gyllenberg D, Marttila M, Sund R, Jokiranta-Olkoniemi E, Sourander A, Gissler M, et al. Temporal changes in the incidence of treated psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders during adolescence: an analysis of two national Finnish birth cohorts. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5:227–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30038-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Martin J, Taylor MJ, Lichtenstein P. Assessing the evidence for shared genetic risks across psychiatric disorders and traits. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1759–74. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. MacKinnon N, Kingsbury M, Mahedy L, Evans J, Colman I. The association between prenatal stress and externalizing symptoms in childhood: evidence from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:100–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.07.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Levine SZ, Kodesh A, Viktorin A, Smith L, Uher R, Reichenberg A, et al. Association of maternal use of folic acid and multivitamin supplements in the periods before and during pregnancy with the risk of autism spectrum disorder in offspring. JAMA Psychiat. 2018;75:176–84. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Yonezawa Y, Ueno F, Obara T, Yamashita T, Ishikuro M, Murakami K, et al. Fruit and vegetable consumption before and during pregnancy and developmental delays in offspring aged 2 years in Japan. Br J Nutr. 2022;127:1250–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114521002154.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hamazaki K, Matsumura K, Tsuchida A, Kasamatsu H, Tanaka T, Ito M, et al. Maternal dietary intake of fish and PUFAs and child neurodevelopment at 6 months and 1 year of age: a nationwide birth cohort-the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS). Am J Clin Nutr. 2020;112:1295–303. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqaa190.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Vecchione R, Wang SW, Rando J, Chavarro JE, Croen LA, Fallin MD, et al. Maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and child autism-related traits: results from two US cohorts. Nutrients. 2022;14(13):2729. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14132729.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Keski-Rahkonen A, Kaprio J, Rissanen A, Virkkunen M, Rose RJ. Breakfast skipping and health-compromising behaviors in adolescents and adults. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2003;57:842–53. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601618.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Asher G, Sassone-Corsi P. Time for food: the intimate interplay between nutrition, metabolism, and the circadian clock. Cell. 2015;161:84–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.03.015.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hoyland A, Dye L, Lawton CL. A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutr Res Rev. 2009;22:220–43. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954422409990175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Shiraishi M, Haruna M, Matsuzaki M. Effects of skipping breakfast on dietary intake and circulating and urinary nutrients during pregnancy. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2019;28:99–105. https://doi.org/10.6133/apjcn.201903_28(1).0014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Dong JY, Ikehara S, Kimura T, Cui M, Kawanishi Y, Kimura T, et al. Skipping breakfast before and during early pregnancy and incidence of gestational diabetes mellitus: the Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2020;111:829–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqaa014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Aizawa M, Murakami K, Takahashi I, Onuma T, Noda A, Ueno F, et al. Skipping breakfast during pregnancy and hypertensive disorders of pregnancy in Japanese women: the Tohoku medical Megabank project birth and three-generation cohort study. Nutr J. 2022;21:71. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-022-00822-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Aizawa M, Murakami K, Takahashi I, Onuma T, Noda A, Ueno F, et al. Association between frequency of breakfast intake before and during pregnancy and infant birth weight: the Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2023;23:268. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-023-05603-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Kuriyama S, Metoki H, Kikuya M, Obara T, Ishikuro M, Yamanaka C, et al. Cohort profile: Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study (TMM BirThree Cohort Study): rationale, progress and perspective. Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49:18–19m. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyz169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Sasaki S, Takahashi T, Iitoi Y, Iwase Y, Kobayashi M, Ishihara J, et al. Food and nutrient intakes assessed with dietary records for the validation study of a self-administered food frequency questionnaire in JPHC Study Cohort I. J Epidemiol. 2003;13(Suppl):S23–50. https://doi.org/10.2188/jea.13.1sup_23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Tsugane S, Kobayashi M, Sasaki S, JPHC. Validity of the self-administered food frequency questionnaire used in the 5-year follow-up survey of the JPHC Study Cohort I: Comparison with dietary records for main nutrients. J Epidemiol. 2003;13(Suppl):S51-6. https://doi.org/10.2188/jea.13.1sup_51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Singh A, Yeh CJ, Boone Blanchard SB. Ages and Stages Questionnaire: a global screening scale. Bol Med Hosp Infant Mex. 2017;74:5–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bmhimx.2016.07.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Mezawa H, Aoki S, Nakayama SF, Nitta H, Ikeda N, Kato K, et al. Psychometric profile of the ages and stages questionnaires, Japanese translation. Pediatr Int. 2019;61:1086–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/ped.13990.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Squires J, Twombly E, As BDB. Q®−3 User’s guide. 3rd ed. Baltimore: Brookes; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Murakami K, Noda A, Ishikuro M, Obara T, Ueno F, Onuma T, et al. Maternal postnatal bonding disorder and developmental delays in children: the Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2023;26:219–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-023-01298-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SLT, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32:959–76. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291702006074.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Furukawa TA, Kawakami N, Saitoh M, Ono Y, Nakane Y, Nakamura Y, et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2008;17:152–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Sakurai K, Nishi A, Kondo K, Yanagida K, Kawakami N. Screening performance of K6/K10 and other screening instruments for mood and anxiety disorders in Japan. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;65:434–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2011.02236.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Okajima I, Nakajima S, Kobayashi M, Inoue Y. Development and validation of the Japanese version of the Athens Insomnia Scale. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;67:420–5. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Soldatos CR, Dikeos DG, Paparrigopoulos TJ. The diagnostic validity of the Athens Insomnia Scale. J Psychosom Res. 2003;55:263–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00604-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Yoshida K, Yamashita H, Conroy S, Marks M, Kumar C. A Japanese version of Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale: factor structure, longitudinal changes and links with maternal mood during the early postnatal period in Japanese mothers. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2012;15:343–52. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00737-012-0291-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Matsunaga A, Takauma F, Tada K, Kitamura T. Discrete category of mother-to-infant bonding disorder and its identification by the Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale: A study in Japanese mothers of a 1-month-old. Early Hum Dev. 2017;111:1–5 (https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378378217300452).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Willett W, Stampfer MJ. Total energy-intake – implications for epidemiologic analyses. Am J Epidemiol. 1986;124:17–27. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114366.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology; 2005–(Japanese). Standard tables of food composition in japan fifth revised and enlarged edition. https://www.mext.go.jp/b_menu/shingi/gijyutu/gijyutu3/toushin/05031802.htm. Accessed 17 June 2023.

  37. Persson P, Rossin-Slater M. Family Ruptures, Stress, and the Mental Health of the Next Generation. Am Econ Rev. 2018;108(4):1214–52. https://www.aeaweb.org/articles?id=10.1257/aer.20141406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Shimazu T, Kuriyama S, Hozawa A, Ohmori K, Sato Y, Nakaya N, et al. Dietary patterns and cardiovascular disease mortality in Japan: a prospective cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(3):600–9. https://academic.oup.com/ije/article/36/3/600/652363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. de Vries JIP, Visser GHA, Mulder EJH, Prechtl HFR. Diurnal and other variations in fetal movement and heart-rate patterns at 20–22 weekes. Early Hum Dev. 1987;15:333–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-3782(87)90029-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Maehara K, Morokuma S, Nakahara K, Okawa H, Kato K. A study on the association between eye movements and regular mouthing movements (RMMs) in normal fetuses between 24 to 39 weeks of gestation. PLoS One. 2020;15(5):e0233909. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233909.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Bueno C, Menna-Barreto L. Development of sleep/wake, activity and temperature rhythms in newborns maintained in a neonatal intensive care unit and the impact of feeding schedules. Infant Behav Dev. 2016;44:21–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hsu CN, Tain YL. Light and circadian signaling pathway in pregnancy: programming of adult health and disease. Int J Mol Sci. 2020;21(6):2232. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21062232.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Nakahara K, Michikawa T, Morokuma T, Hamada N, Ogawa M, Kato K, et al. Association of physical activity and sleep habits during pregnancy with autistic spectrum disorder in 3-year-old infants. Commun Med. 2022;2:35. https://doi.org/10.1038/s43856-022-00101-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Miike T, Toyoura M, Tonooka S, Konishi Y, Oniki K, Saruwatari J, et al. Neonatal irritable sleep-wake rhythm as a predictor of autism spectrum disorders. Neurobiol Sleep Circadian Rhythms. 2020;9:100053. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbscr.2020.100053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Miyake K, Mochizuki K, Kushima M, Shinohara R, Horiuchi S, Otawa S, et al. Maternal protein intake in early pregnancy and child development at age 3 years. Pediatr Res. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-022-02435-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Elango R, Ball RO. Protein and amino acid requirements during pregnancy. Adv Nutr. 2016;7:839S-S844. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.115.011817.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Carson C, Kelly Y, Kurinczuk JJ, Sacker A, Redshaw M, Quigley MA. Effect of pregnancy planning and fertility treatment on cognitive outcomes in children at ages 3 and 5: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. 2011;343:d4473 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21791498.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Lugo-Gil J, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Family resources and parenting quality: links to children’s cognitive development across the first 3 years. Child Dev. 2008;79:1065–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Doyle O, Harmon CP, Heckman JJ, Tremblay RE. Investing in early human development: timing and economic efficiency. Econ Hum Biol. 2009;7:1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Kotulak R. Inside the brain: revolutionary discoveries of how the mind works. Prev Med. 1998;27:246–7. https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1998.0281.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. Dietary guidelines for pregnant and nursing women starting before pregnancy. 2021 (in Japanese). https://www.cfa.go.jp/policies/boshihoken/shokuji/. Accessed 4 Dec 2023.

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED), Japan (grant number, JP17km0105001, JP21tm0124005).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MA designed the study, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript; KM and SK reviewed the manuscript; TO, JS, MI, and SK contributed to data collection; All authors provided critical feedback and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Keiko Murakami.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The TMM BirThree Cohort Study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tohoku University Tohoku Medical Megabank Organization (2013–1-103–1).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

 Supplementary Table 1. Comparison of characteristics of included and excluded participants. Supplementary Table 2. Association between frequency of breakfast intake during pregnancy and each of the five domains of ASQ-3 at 2 years of age. Supplementary Table 3. Association between frequency of breakfast intake during pregnancy and each of the five domains of ASQ-3 at 3.5 years of age. Supplementary Table 4. Analyses stratified by maternal psychological distress, insomnia symptoms and employment status in the association between the frequency of maternal breakfast intake and developmental delays in children at 2 years of age. Supplementary Table 5. Characteristics of participants by age of developmental delays.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Aizawa, M., Murakami, K., Takahashi, I. et al. Association between frequency of breakfast intake before and during pregnancy and developmental delays in children: the Tohoku Medical Megabank Project Birth and Three-Generation Cohort Study. Nutr J 22, 66 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-023-00901-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-023-00901-5

Keywords