Skip to main content

Stunting, selenium deficiency and anemia are associated with poor cognitive performance in preschool children from rural Ethiopia

Abstract

Background

Anthropometric characteristics and iron status affect cognitive performance in children. In addition, selenium can influence cognitive outcomes; protection of the brain from oxidative stress and its role in thyroid hormone metabolism are putative mechanisms.

Methods

To investigate their association with cognitive performance, anthropometric indicators, iron biomarkers, and serum selenium of children (n = 541) of 54-60mo of age from rural Ethiopia were assessed. Cognitive assessment was conducted with the administration of two reasoning subtests of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence and the school readiness test.

Results

Stunting was found in 41.4 % of children, 28.7 % were underweight, and 6.3 % were wasted. The mean score of stunted children was lower than that of non-stunted children on non-verbal reasoning (7.0 ± 3.2vs7.9 ± 3.1; p = 0.01) and the school readiness tests (4.3 ± 2.2 vs 3.3 ± 2.1; p < 0.001). Compared to non-anemic children, anemic children had lower score for the verbal reasoning test (9.5 ± 1.7 vs 8.9 ± 2.2; p = 0.02). However, except for hemoglobin, none of the iron biomarkers had significant associations with the cognitive score of the study children (p > 0.05). Selenium deficient children had lower scores on all cognitive tests than normal children (p < 0.05).

Conclusion

The present study finding linking chronic undernutrition and micronutrient deficiency to cognitive deficits suggests the need for designing effective intervention programmes to control for protein energy malnutrition and micronutrient deficiency and address cognitive development in children.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Protein energy malnutrition and micronutrient deficiencies are major concerns among children from low income countries. Worldwide, 25 % of children under 5 years are stunted, 15 % are underweight and 8 % are wasted [1]. Sub-Saharan Africa has the second largest number of stunted and underweight children next to south Asia [2]. In Ethiopia, 40.4 % of children under the age of five years are stunted, 25 % are underweight, and 9 % are wasted [3]. Iron deficiency is one of the world’s common single nutrient deficiencies [4] and about a quarter of the world’s populations are affected by anemia [5]. Selenium (Se) deficiency is less commonly studied and reported than anemia. Yet more than half a billion people in the world [6] and about 60 % of children from the northwest of Ethiopia [7] are reported to be Se deficient.

Protein-energy malnutrition during early childhood constrains structural and functional development of the brain and affects cognitive function [8]. Effects of early stunting on cognitive deficits can persist throughout later life [9] and successive generations [10]. Iron plays an important role for the synthesis of neurotransmitters and myelination of neurons [11]. Deficiency of iron has been associated with poorer cognitive performance in children and was implicated in long lasting effects even after treatment for iron deficiency [12, 13]. Anemia is also a significant risk factor for cognitive deficit [14].

Brain oxidative stress may be another significant contributor to cognitive deficits [15]. Selenium, a potent antioxidant micronutrient [16], may impact cognitive function by protecting the brain from oxidative damage [17]. In addition, Se is known to influence the process of synaptogenesis, myelination, and neuronal cell differentiation by regulating thyroid hormones [18]. Several researchers have investigated the association between Se nutrition and cognitive performance or protective effects of Se against cognitive deficit [1923]. However, studies regarding the association of Se status and cognitive performance to date have been limited to animal models or related to age-induced memory loss in humans. This study investigated the association of anthropometric characteristics, iron biomarkers, and serum Se with cognitive performance of preschool children from rural Ethiopia.

Materials and methods

Study subjects

As part of a randomized control trial(ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT013496) investigating the effect of iodized salt on cognitive development of children, sixty rural kebeles from randomly selected six zones (West Gojjam, East Gojjam, North Gondar, North Wollo, South Wollo, and Wagehmera) of the Amhara region first were randomly selected. Next, one village was randomly selected from each of the sixty rural kebeles. A house-to-house census was conducted to register all children 6–60 mo old. Each child’s age was obtained from the immunization card or from the parents or guardian. An event calendar, containing dates and year of local or national festivals, traditions, and other major events, was prepared as an aide to help parents correctly report the age of their child. To avoid variation of cognitive performance due to difference in the age of children, the children were further classified into three groups as 6–10, 18–22 and 54-60mo. In the present analysis, baseline data were included from children (N = 541), 54-60mo of age from 26 villages randomly selected out of the original 60 villages.

Anthropometric measurement

Height was measured in an erect position using a calibrated wooden height-measuring board (Stadiometer, Shorr Productions, Olney, MD, USA) with a sliding head bar while children were barefoot. Their height was measured in duplicate and recorded to the nearest 0.1 cm or in triplicate whenever the deviation between the first two measurements was > 0.5 cm and the average of all measurements was taken. Weight was measured using a battery-powered digital scale (Mother-child scale, Max capacity 440 lb (200 kg), precision 0.1 kg (TANITA WB-100, Tanita Corporation, Arlington Heights, IL,USA) while children wore light clothes with empty pockets and no shoes. The weight scale was calibrated at least twice a day. Weight was measured twice and recorded to the nearest 0.1 kg and the average was taken.

Blood collection and analysis

Experienced nurses drew blood. Blood was drawn from the antecubital vein with disposable butterfly vacutainer needles (BD Vacutainer butterfly needles, 23G, 3/4", Franklin Lakes, NJ, USA) using standard safety measures. Hemoglobin (Hb) level was measured using a digital Hemocue (201). Serum ferritin was analyzed using a clinical analyzer, Electrochemilumenescense Immuno Assay (ECLIA) Elecsys2010 analyzer, Cobas e 411. Soluble transferrin receptor and α1-acid glycoprotein (AGP) was determined by a clinical chemistry analyzer Cobas Integra 400 system. The detailed procedure followed for the analysis of Hb, serum ferritin, serum soluble transferrin receptor (sTFR), and AGP is reported elsewhere [24]. Duplicate frozen serum samples were shipped to the laboratory of the Department of Nutritional Sciences at Oklahoma State University, USA in an insulated box with dry ice for Se analysis.

Serum Se was analyzed by inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometer (ICP-MS) (PerkinElmer, ELAN9000, Norwalk, CT). Serum samples were diluted in 0.1 % trace metal grade HNO3 (Fisher Scientific, Fair Lawn, NJ). Working standards for Se were freshly prepared by diluting 1000 ppm pure Se stock solution (PerkinElmer) in a solution of 0.1 % HNO3 and 0.5 % Triton-X-100 (Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO). Gallium (PerkinElmer) was used as an internal standard. A reference standard of freeze-dried human serum (Utak Laboratories, Inc., Valencia, CA) was used to verify the method performance. The reference standard was analyzed within every twenty samples to check the performance of the instrument. The Se value (105.13-112.66 μg/l) of the reference serum from the ICP-MS analysis was near the target value (111 μg/l) set by the manufacturer. The inter- and intra- assay coefficient of variation for the analysis was 4.3 % and 1.2 %, respectively. Trace mineral free gloves, plastic ware and pipette tips were used during analysis of the serum samples. A serum Se concentration below 70.0 μg/l was defined as Se deficiency [25].

Assessment of cognitive function

The cognitive assessment included two subtests of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-III) [26] and a 25-item school readiness test. The first two are considered sufficient for research purposes to measure fluid intelligence: Matrix Reasoning measures nonverbal analytic reasoning based on the ability to complete patterns and analogies; Similarities measures verbal reasoning based on the ability to form verbal concepts. The school readiness test assesses the acquisition of preliteracy and prenumeracy skills in preparation for primary school. These tests assign 1 point for every correct answer and 0 for incorrect answers. The cognitive tests were translated to the local language (Amharic) and back-translated to English to check for accuracy of translation. The tests were adapted to the context of the study children by replacing items and expressions in the original cognitive tests that are not familiar in the study area with elements common in the locality but with similar level of complexity and meaning. The cognitive tests were pretested in non-study children from neighboring rural kebeles and necessary corrections were made. The tests were conducted in Amharic. Psychology graduates were trained by experienced researchers to administer the tests in a child-friendly manner. Inter-rater reliability yielded highly significant correlations (p < 0.001). Before starting the test, research assistants made sure that children were at ease and relaxed so as to avoid fear and shyness. The tests were conducted mainly in the child’s home. We present here baseline standardized WPPSI scores and raw scores of the school readiness test.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis of data was performed using SPSS for Windows (v18). Normality of data was checked by Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. WHO Anthro software was used for analysis of anthropometric measures. Height-for-age, weight-for-age, and weight-for-height were used to classify the study children into categories of nutritional status using the WHO Multicenter Growth Reference [27]. Children below −2 height-for-age z-score (HAZ), −2 weight-for-age z-score (WAZ), and −2 weight-for-height z-score (WHZ) were classified as stunted, underweight, and wasted, respectively. Pearson correlation was conducted to study bivariate correlations. Scores for the three cognitive tests of Se deficient and normal children, stunted and non-stunted, underweight and normal, and anemic and non-anemic children were compared using Student’s t-test. To see the effect of multiple nutrient deficiencies on cognitive performance compared to single nutrient deficiencies, children were classified into groups and their cognitive scores were compared using one way analysis of variance. The least significant difference method was used for mean separation. A probability level of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Multiple regression was conducted to identify the most important variables (among micronutrients and anthropometric characteristics) influencing cognitive performance of the children. In addition, linear regression analysis was also done to study the association of several factors including mother’s education, family assets, child’s sex, dietary diversity, presence of goiter, urinary iodine, serum thyroxin, serum triodothyronine, serum thyroglobulin, and serum zinc with cognitive scores by children. However, they were either not significant or explained very small percentage of variability.

Ethics

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the National Health Research Ethics Review Committee at the Ethiopian Science and Technology Commission, and the Institutional Review Boards at McGill University, Canada and Oklahoma State University, USA. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents or guardians of the study children.

Results

The study children (n = 541) had male to female ratio of 1.02:1. The mean age of the children was 56.9 ± 1.8 mo with a range of 54 to 60 mo. Summary of HAZ, WAZ, and WHZ of study children is shown in Table 1. Bilateral edema was not evident in any of the children. Overall, 41.4 % (n = 224) of children were stunted, 28.7 % (n = 155) were underweight, and 6.3 % (n = 34) were wasted (Table 1).

Table 1 Nutritional status of children (n = 541) from the Amhara region, Ethiopia, October 2011-May 2012

Our previous analyses indicated that anemia was present in 13.6 % of children. Based on serum ferritin, 9.1 % of children were iron deficient. Iron deficiency anemia (IDA) was seen in 5.3 % of children and 14.2 % of children had elevated sTFR (>5.0 mg/l) [24]. Of the total sample, 58.4 % (n = 316) of children had serum Se concentrations below70 μg/l.

A summary of cognitive scores of the study children is depicted in Table 2. The mean score of stunted children was significantly lower than mean score of non-stunted children for the non-verbal reasoning (7.0 ± 3.2vs7.9 ± 3.1; p = 0.01) and the school readiness tests (4.3 ± 2.2 vs 3.3 ± 2.1; p < 0.001). In addition, HAZ was weakly but significantly correlated with the verbal reasoning test (r = 0.13; p < 0.01), and WAZ was correlated with the school readiness tests (r = 0.18; p < 0.001). However, mean score of underweight and normal weight children or children with wasting and their normal counterparts was not significantly different (p > 0.05). Comparison of cognitive scores of the study children by their nutritional status is shown in Fig. 1

Table 2 Cognitive scores by children from the Amhara region, Ethiopia, October 2011-May 2012
Fig. 1
figure 1

Comparison of cognitive scores of preschool children by nutritional status. * the difference is significant at p < 0.05, **the difference is significant at p < 0.01

Preliminary correlations showed that mother’s education, family assets, dietary diversity, iodine status (goiter, urinary iodine, serum thyroxin, serum triodothyronine, and serum thyroglobulin), and serum zinc were not associated with test scores. No sex-related difference with respect to Se status, anthropometric charactertics, iron status, hemoglobin level, or cognitive performance was detected. Compared to non-anemic children, anemic children had significantly lower scores for the similarity test (9.5 ± 1.7 vs 8.9 ± 2.2; p = 0.02). However, none of the iron biomarkers had significant associations with cognitive score of the study children (P > 0.05). Children with low serum Se had significantly lower cognitive scores for all cognitive tests. Means were 7.1 ± 3.1 vs 8.0 ± 3.2; p = 0.005 for the matrix reasoning test; 9.0 ± 1.9 vs 9.6 ± 1.6; p = 0.02 for the similarity test; and 3.5 ± 2.4 vs 4.2 ± 2.5; p = 0.02 for the school readiness test (Fig. 2). There was no significant difference (p > 0.05) between the level of serum Se in stunted and non-stunted or anemic and non-anemic children. Compared to children with one or more micronutrient deficiency (anemia, iron deficiency, Se deficiency) or stunting, normal children had significantly (p < 0.05) higher cognitive scores in all of the cognitive tests. In addition, children with coexisting burden of anemia, iron and Se deficiency, and stunting had lower cognitive scores in the similarity and matrix reasoning tests (p < 0.05). Children with two or more coexisting nutrient deficiencies scored significantly lower than children affected by single nutrient deficiency in all of the cognitive tests (p < 0.05). In addition, hemoglobin in the similarity test (β = 0.32, p = 0.001) and serum Se in the matrix reasoning (β = 0.39, p = 0.002) and school readiness tests (β = 0.357, p = 0.04) were the most important variables to affect cognitive performance of the study children.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Comparison of cognitive scores between selenium deficient and selenium adequate preschool children.* the difference is significant at p < 0.05, **the difference is significant at p < 0.01

Discussion

This study investigated the association of anthropometric indicators, iron biomarkers, and serum Se with cognitive performance of preschool children from the rural areas of the Amhara region, Ethiopia. The study children had a high prevalence of stunting and Se deficiency. Anemia was a mild public health problem and there was low prevalence of IDA. In the present study children, stunting, Se deficiency, and anemia were associated with poor cognitive performance.

Stunting is the consequence of several macro- and micronutrient deficiencies and poor health. In the present study, stunted children had lower cognitive scores for all of the psychological tests. Similar to the findings of the present study, Mendez & Adair [16] found significantly lower test scores in stunted Filipino children than their non-stunted counterparts. Likewise, in India, stunted children performed poorly and had much lower scores than adequately nourished children on cognitive tests [8]. In addition, in children from developing countries such as Ecuador [28], Vietnam [29], Brazil [30], Malaysia [31], and Cambodia [11], stunting was a significant predictor of lower cognitive test scores. Results of the present study show importantly, that in the year before entering primary school, stunted children are at a disadvantage regarding school readiness and the verbal and nonverbal reasoning skills needed for education in early grades.

According to the Cost of Hunger Study [32], in Ethiopia, 16 % of repetitions in primary school was associated with stunting and stunted children achieve 1.1 years less in school education compared to their normal counterparts. In addition, in that study, it was estimated that child stunting in Ethiopia costs about 16.2 % of the country’s Gross Domestic Product suggesting the enormous challenge of child undernutrition on economic and human capital development on the country and the need for appropriate early childhood interventions to halt the cyclic patterns of poverty.

In the present study children, no significant difference was found between cognitive scores of iron deficient and normal children. In addition, none of the iron biomarkers had significant association with cognitive performance of children. However, results from other researchers show that iron deficiency was associated with poor cognitive performance attributed to alteration on dopamine functioning [33, 34], reduced brain cytochrome c concentration, lower fatty acid in the brain thus myelination [35], and disrupting neurochemical profile of the hippocampus [36]. Iron may have had weak effects here because most of the children in the present study were iron sufficient. Compared to non-anemic children, anemic children however had significantly lower cognitive scores for the verbal reasoning test. Similarly, other studies also found significant relationships between Hb concentration and cognitive performance in children [14, 37, 38]. However in addition to poor iron status, anemia typically is caused by several socioeconomic conditions and dietary factors [39] thus the difference in cognitive performance of the present study anemic and non-anemic preschool children may not be attributed to iron status alone.

Selenium levels in serum were found to be associated with all three cognitive measures. This study makes a novel contribution to the literature as there is a dearth of studies on the role of Se in cognitive development of children. The brain is one of the organs with high concentration of Se. In addition, in cases of Se deficiency, it is the organ that remains Se replete the longest. It is also the first organ to obtain adequate level of Se during dietary intake suggesting the important role of Se in brain functions [40]. Several seleno-proteins such as GPx, selenoprotein P, selenoprotein W, and thioredoxin reductase were detected in the brain that modulate and support the activity of the brain by protecting it against oxidative damage [41, 42]. Brain oxidative stress is a casual factor for cognitive impairment [19] and protection against the risk of cognitive deficits by selenoproteins is associated with their role in the antioxidation system [17]. In addition, the effect of Se on cognition is mediated through its role on thyroid metabolism [18].

Although a substantial body of evidence has been compiled on the relationship between Se on cognition of adult humans and animals, little deals with children. In elderly Chinese people, Se deficiency was considered as a risk factor for lower cognitive test outcomes [20]. Similarly, in a 9-year cohort study of community-dwelling French elderly, a decrease in plasma Se was significantly associated with a decline in cognitive performance [19]. Another 4 years follow-up study showed that subjects with low level of Se had an increased risk of cognitive decline [43]. In addition, Se deficiency independently was associated with lower cognitive performance [21]. In rural Bangladesh, maternal red cell Se status during pregnancy was positively associated with children’s developmental measures such as language comprehension and psychomotor development indicating the influence of prenatal Se nutrition on children’s psychomotor development [44].

Conclusion

In general, the present study children had high prevalence of chronic and acute undernutrition and Se deficiency, mild anemia but low iron deficiency and IDA. Stunting, anemia and Se deficiency were associated with poor cognitive performance. Therefore, identifying the etiological factors and designing appropriate interventions for accelerated stunting reduction and anemia control is vital. The present study children were from subsistence farming households and consume foods grown in their local area. In such settings, Se status of populations is mainly dictated by the level of available Se in the soil [45] which again is governed by the chemistry of the soil [46]. Thus, identifying main soil physicochemical factors that influence Se phytoavailability and knowledge of soil Se concentration would be an important step to design an intervention aimed at increasing Se concentration in staple crops and control Se deficiency. The present study children were from households of similar educational status and occupation. However, there is ample evidence that children’s cognitive outcome can be affected by other factors that were not captured in this analysis, including maternal depression and poverty [47], poor care and inadequate cognitive stimulation [48], parenting style [49], and parasitic infection [50]. In addition, it has been reported that the physical home environment such as housing conditions, risk of exposure to toxicants, chronic and acute noise exposure, and crowding can significantly affect the cognitive performance of children [51].

Abbreviations

AGP:

α1-acid glycoprotein

ECLIA:

Electrochemilumenescense Immuno Assay

HAZ:

height-for-age z-score

IDA:

Iron deficiency anemia

sTFR:

soluble transferrin receptor

WAZ:

weight-for-age z-score

WHO:

World Health Organization

WHZ:

weight-for-height z-score

WPPSI:

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence

References

  1. UNICEF. The state of the world’s children. New York, NY: UNICEF. http://www.unicef.org/sowc (2014). Accessed 04 Nov 2015.

  2. Stevens GA, Finucane MM, Paciorek CJ, Flaxman SR, White RA, Donner AJ, et al. Trends in mild, moderate, and severe stunting and underweight, and progress towards MDG 1 in 141 developing countries: a systematic analysis of population representative data. Lancet. 2012;380:824–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Ethiopian Central Statistical Agency: Ethiopian Mini Demographic and Health Survey. Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. http://www.unicef.org/ethiopia/Mini_DHS_2014__Final_Report.Pdf (2014). Accessed 20 Nov 2015.

  4. Stoltzfus RJ. Iron deficiency: global prevalence and consequences. Food Nutr Bull. 2003;24:S99–S103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. WHO. Worldwide prevalence of anaemia 1993–2005. WHO Global data base on anaemia. Geneva: WHO. http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/43894/1/9789241596657_eng.pdf (2008). Accessed 21 Nov 2015.

  6. Haug A, Graham RD, Christophersen OA, Lyons GH. How to use the world’s scarce selenium resources efficiently to increase the selenium concentration in food. Microb Ecol Health Dis. 2007;19:209–28.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Amare B, Moges B, Fantahun B, Tafess K, Woldeyohannes D, Yismaw G, et al. Micronutrient levels and nutritional status of school children living in Northwest Ethiopia. Nutrition J. 2010;11:108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Kar BR, Rao SL, Chandramouli BA. Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition. Behav Brain Funct. 2008;4:1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Mendez MA, Adair LS. Severity and timing of stunting in the first two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. J Nutr. 1999;129:1555–62.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Walker SP, Chang SM, Wright A, Osmond C, Grantham-McGregor SM. Early childhood stunting is associated with lower developmental levels in the subsequent generation of children. J Nutr. 2015;145:823–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Perignon M, Fiorentino M, Kuong K, Burja K, Parker M, Sisokhom S, et al. Stunting, poor iron status and parasite infection are significant risk factors for lower cognitive performance in Cambodian school-aged children. PLoS One. 2014;9, e112605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Smith JB. Double burden of iron deficiency in infancy and low socioeconomic status: a longitudinal analysis of cognitive test scores to age 19 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160:1108–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Kordas K, Lopez P, Rosado JL, Vargas GG, Rico JA, Ronquillo D, et al. Blood lead, anemia, and short stature are independently associated with cognitive performance in Mexican school children. J Nutr. 2004;134:363–71.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Cardoso BR, Apolinário D, da Silva Bandeira V, Busse AL, Magaldi RM, Jacob-Filho W, et al. Effects of Brazil nut consumption on selenium status and cognitive performance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled pilot trial. Euro J Nutr. 2015; doi:10.1007/s00394-014-0829-2.

  16. Mistry HD, Kurlak LO, Young SD, Briley AL, Broughton Pipkin F, Baker PN, et al. Maternal selenium, copper and zinc concentrations in pregnancy associated with small‐for‐gestational‐age infants. Matern Child Nutr. 2014;10:327–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Berr C, Arnaud J, Akbaraly TN. Selenium and cognitive impairment: A brief‐review based on results from the EVA study. Biofactors. 2012;38:139–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Sher L. Role of thyroid hormones in the effects of selenium on mood, behavior, and cognitive function. Med Hypotheses. 2001;57:480–3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Akbaraly NT, Hininger-Favier I, Carriere I, Arnaud J, Gourlet V, Roussel AM, et al. Plasma selenium over time and cognitive decline in the elderly. Epidemiology. 2007;18:52–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gao S, Jin Y, Unverzagt FW, Liang C, Hall KS, Cao J, et al. Selenium level and depressive symptoms in a rural elderly Chinese cohort. BMC Psychiatry. 2001;12:72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Gassió R, Artuch R, Vilaseca MA, Fusté E, Colome R, Campistol J. Cognitive functions and the antioxidant system in phenylketonuric patients. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:426–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ishrat T, Parveen K, Khan MM, Khuwaja G, Khan MB, Yousuf S, et al. Selenium prevents cognitive decline and oxidative damage in rat model of streptozotocin-induced experimental dementia of Alzheimer's type. Brain Res. 2009;1281:117–27.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Qin F, Yuan H, Nie J, Cao Y, Tong J. Effects of nano-selenium on cognition performance of mice exposed in 1800 MHz radiofrequency fields. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu. 2014;43:16–21.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Gashu D, Stoecker BJ, Adish A, Haki GD, Bougma K, Marquis GS. Ethiopian preschool children consuming predominantly unrefined plant-based diet have low prevalence of iron deficiency anaemia. Public Health Nutr. In press.

  25. Sauberlich HE. Laboratory tests for the assessment of nutritional status. 2nd ed. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  27. WHO Multicenter Growth Reference Study Group. WHO child growth standards based on length/height, weight and age. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 2006;450:76–85.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Grandjean P, Harari R, Barr DB, Debes F. Pesticide exposure and stunting as independent predictors of neurobehavioral deficits in Ecuadorian school children. Pediatrics. 2006;117:e546–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hall A, Khanh LN, Son TH, Dung NQ, Lansdown RG, Dar DT, et al. An association between chronic undernutrition and educational test scores in Vietnamese children. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2001;55:801–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Brito GNO, de Onis MD. Growth status, behavior and neuropsychological performance: a study of Brazilian school age children. ArqNeuropsiquiatr. 2004;62:949–54.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Shariff ZM, Bond JT, Johnson NE. Nutrition and educational achievement of urban primary school children in Malaysia. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2000;9:264–73.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. AUC, NEPAD, UNECA, WFP (2012) The Cost of Hunger in Africa: Social and economic impact of child undernutrition in Egypt, Ethiopia, Swaziland and Uganda. Addis Ababa: UNECA. http://www.uneca.org/sites/default/files/PublicationFiles/CoHA%20English_web.pdf. Accessed 18 Nov 2015.

  33. Lozoff B. Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. J Nutr. 2011;141:740S–6S.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Konofal E, Lecendreux M, Arnulf I. Iron deficiency in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158:1113–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Georgieff MK. The role of iron in neurodevelopment: fetal iron deficiency and the developing hippocampus. Biochem Soc Trans. 2008;36:1267–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Rao R, Tkac I, Townsend EL, Gruetter R, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency alters the neurochemical profile of the developing rat hippocampus. J Nutr. 2003;133:3215–21.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Roy A, Bellinger D, Hu H, Modali R, Schwartz J, Wright R, et al. Lead exposure, hemoglobin and intelligence quotient: effect modification by dopamine receptor D2 Taq IA polymorphism among children in Chennai. India Epidemiol. 2009;20:S77–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Sungthong R, Mo‐suwan L, Chongsuvivatwong V. Effects of haemoglobin and serum ferritin on cognitive function in school children. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2002;11:117–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Pasricha SR, Black J, Muthayya S, Shet A, Bhat V, Nagaragi S, et al. Determinants of anemia among young children in rural India. Pediatrics. 2010;126:e140–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Bourre JM. Effects of nutrients (in food) on the structure and function of the nervous system: update on dietary requirements for brain. Part 1: micronutrients. J Nutr Health Aging. 2006;10:377–85.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Schweizer U, Bräuer AU, Köhrle J, Nitsch R, Savaskan NE. Selenium and brain function: a poorly recognized liaison. Brain Res Rev. 2004;45:164–78.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Chen J, Berry MJ. Selenium and selenoproteins in the brain and brain diseases. J Neurochem. 2003;86:1–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Berr C, Balansard B, Arnaud J, Roussel AM, Alpérovitch A. Cognitive decline is associated with systemic oxidative stress: the EVA study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:1285–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Skröder HM, Hamadani JD, Tofail F, Persson LÅ, Vahter ME, Kippler MJ. Selenium status in pregnancy influences children's cognitive function at 1.5 years of age. Clin Nutr. 2014;34:923–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Navarro-Alarcon M, Cabrera-Vique C. Selenium in food and the human body. Sci Total Environ. 2008;400:115–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Fordyce FM, Guangdi Z, Green K, Xinping L. Soil, grain and water chemistry in relation to human selenium-responsive diseases in Enshi District. China Appl Geochem. 2000;15:117–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Petterson SM, Albers AB. Effects of poverty and maternal depression on early child development. Child Dev. 2001;72:1794–813.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Kiernan KE, Huerta MC. Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children’s cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. Br J Sociol. 2008;59:783–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Berkman DS, Lescano AG, Gilman RH, Lopez SL, Black MM. Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection during infancy on cognition in late childhood: a follow-up study. Lancet. 2002;359:564–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Evans GW. Child development and the physical environment. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;57:423–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank health extension workers and study participants; Dr. Frances Aboud for training the data collection team, helping in the analysis and interpretation of the data, and commenting the draft manuscript; Tizita Lemma and Hussein Mohammed for supervising and managing the data collection; Sandra Peterson for assistance with the serum Se analysis; Ethiopian Public Health Institute for the analysis of iron biomarkers. Micronutrient Initiative, the German Academic Exchange Service, and Addis Ababa University for their financial support. Dr. Dawd GASHU Adem is a fellow of the Norman E. Borlaug Leadership Enhancement in Agriculture Program funded by USAID. Support for this research was provided in part by the Borlaug Leadership Enhancement in Agriculture Program (Borlaug LEAP) through a grant to the University of California-Davis by the United States Agency for International Development. The opinions expressed herein are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of USAID.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dawd Gashu.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

DG, BJS, KB and GSM developed the study design; DG and KB supervised the field work; DG and BJS conducted the biochemical analysis; DG, BJS, and GSM analyzed and interpreted the data. All authors contributed to manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gashu, D., Stoecker, B.J., Bougma, K. et al. Stunting, selenium deficiency and anemia are associated with poor cognitive performance in preschool children from rural Ethiopia. Nutr J 15, 38 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-016-0155-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12937-016-0155-z

Keywords