Open Access
Open Peer Review

This article has Open Peer Review reports available.

How does Open Peer Review work?

No evidence of differential effects of SFA, MUFA or PUFA on post-ingestive satiety and energy intake: a randomised trial of fatty acid saturation

  • Caroline M Strik1,
  • Fiona E Lithander2,
  • Anne-Thea McGill3,
  • Alastair K MacGibbon4,
  • Brian H McArdle5 and
  • Sally D Poppitt6Email author
Nutrition Journal20109:24

DOI: 10.1186/1475-2891-9-24

Received: 6 October 2009

Accepted: 24 May 2010

Published: 24 May 2010

Abstract

Background

High fat diets have long been associated with weight gain and obesity, and the weak satiety response elicited in response to dietary lipids is likely to play a role. Suppression of appetite and food intake has consistently been shown to be diminished with high fat relative to either high protein or carbohydrate meals. There is however some evidence that the satiating capacity of lipids may be modulated when physicochemical properties are altered, but studies investigating the effect of lipid saturation on appetite have generated inconsistent findings. This study investigated the effects of changes in fatty acid saturation on post-ingestive satiety and energy intake.

Methods

High-fat (HF) test breakfasts (2.0 MJ) containing 26 g lipid were given to 18 healthy, lean men in a 3 treatment randomised cross-over design, each treatment separated by a washout of at least 3 days. The breakfasts were high in saturated (SFA, 65% of total fat), polyunsaturated (PUFA, 76%) or monounsaturated (MUFA, 76%) fatty acids, and comprised 2 savoury muffins. Participants rated appetite sensations using visual analogue scales (VAS) to assess palatability immediately following the meals, and hunger and fullness prior to the HF breakfast and throughout the day. Energy intake was measured by covert weighing of a lunch meal which was served 3.5 h after the breakfast, and from which the participants ate ad libitum.

Results

There was no difference in VAS ratings of pleasantness, visual appearance, smell, taste, aftertaste and overall palatability between the 3 high-fat test breakfasts. However, there was also no differential effect of the 3 treatments on ratings of hunger, fullness, satisfaction or prospective food consumption during the 3.5 h following the breakfast meal and over the full 6 h experiment. Energy and macronutrient intake at lunch also did not differ between treatments (mean, sem; SFA: 5275.9 ± 286.5 kJ; PUFA: 5227.7 ± 403.9 kJ; MUFA: 5215.6 ± 329.5 kJ; P > 0.05). The maximum difference in energy intake between treatments was less than 2%.

Conclusions

There was no evidence of a difference in post-ingestion satiety between high fat meals which differed in saturation profile in this group of lean, healthy men.

Trial Registration

ACTRN12610000193077

Introduction

A high intake of dietary fat has long been implicated in the development of obesity with a positive association between a high-fat (HF), high-energy dense diet and a high body mass index (BMI) [1, 2]. Whether there is a causal relationship between dietary fat and the current high levels of obesity continues to be debated however, [38] as does dietary fat as a primary driver of hyperphagia, or overconsumption, during weight gain. In a number of early studies dietary fat was shown to exert a weaker satiating effect than isoenergetic amounts of either carbohydrate (CHO) or protein in most [911], although not all [12, 13] studies. Possibly factors such as palatability [14], high energy density [10, 11, 1517] and relatively weak oxidative feedback of dietary lipids [15, 1820] may play a role in overconsumption of lipid-rich foods [10], the latter hypothesis having been recently revisited [21]. The inability of lipids to mount a strong postprandial satiety-related hormone response relative to the other macronutrients may also contribute [17, 22, 23].

There is also some evidence to suggest that the association of a HF diet with weight gain and obesity may in part depend on the saturation of the fatty acids (FA) consumed and that unsaturated, and in particular polyunsaturated FAs (PUFA), may be associated with lower adiposity [2426]. Several studies have shown that the rate of postprandial fat oxidation, purported to act as a satiety feedback signal, is negatively related to the degree of saturation [2733]. In early studies degree of saturation was also linked with changes in putative appetite modifiers such as CCK [34], insulin and serotonin [35]. In addition, effects on ad libitum energy intake (EI) have been reported. Whilst there is some evidence of PUFA having the strongest and MUFA the weakest suppression of food intake, clinical studies are inconsistent and variable. A gastrointestinal infusion study showed high-PUFA Intralipid® (a fat emulsion comprising largely soybean oil) and linoleic acid (18:2) to decrease intake when compared with a no-fat saline control, whilst carbon chain length (CCL) -matched stearic acid (C18:0, SFA) and oleic acid (C18:1, MUFA) did not [36]. In a feeding study of CCL-matched fats, both high-PUFA and high-SFA meals decreased food intake when compared with high-MUFA meals [37], and a second infusion study showed lauric acid (C12:0 SFA) but not the longer chain oleic acid (C18:1 MUFA) to decrease food intake relative to a saline control [38]. Conversely, other studies have found less convincing [39, 40] or no evidence of saturation affecting ad libitum EI or appetite ratings [4143], or with response differing by gender [39].

This lack of consensus led to our current study which investigated whether changes in the saturation profile of a high-fat breakfast meal affected postprandial appetite sensations and food consumption at the subsequent meal using a study design which we have previously shown to be sensitive to manipulations in energy and fat content [44]. Based on prior studies we hypothesized that there may be a satiety hierarchy of PUFA > SFA > MUFA when a high fat meal balanced for both energy and total fat content is consumed.

Participants and Methods

Participants

Lean male participants (BMI 18-25 kg/m2), aged between 18-55 years were recruited into this intervention trial through poster, newspaper and electronic advertisement. Body weight and height were measured whilst fasted on 2 consecutive occasions and mean BMI recorded. Participants were from the local community including tertiary institutions. Exclusion criteria were self-reported current or previous history of overweight or obesity, current or recent history (previous 6 months) of dieting including commercial weight loss programs or weight loss surgery, eating disorders or significant restraint [45], smoking, hypertension, cardiovascular disease, diabetes mellitus (type I or II), and any significant metabolic, endocrine or gastrointestinal disease. Normal lipid profile, full blood count and fasting blood glucose levels were also ensured at screen. None of the participants were taking medications known to affect appetite or weight regulation. Written consent was obtained from each of the participants. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Northern Regional Ethics Committee, Auckland, New Zealand.

Experimental design

This was a randomised, 3 treatment cross-over trial where the effects of a HF test breakfast supplemented with SFA, PUFA or MUFA were assessed through subjective visual analogue scales (VAS) and ad libitum EI at a single lunch meal. The primary objective of measurement of food intake at the lunch meal was not revealed to the participants prior to the study. All participants attended the Human Nutrition Unit (HNU) on three separate occasions between which they returned home for a washout period of at least 3 days where they were asked to resume their habitual diet and exercise pattern. Participants were asked to abstain from alcohol and strenuous physical activity for 24 hours prior to the study-day, and to complete a 24-hour dietary recall and physical activity questionnaires. Physical activity was estimated using a questionnaire evaluating activities according to duration and intensity using the PEPSA (Physical Exercise Programme for Sedentary Adults) points system [46] to assess whether differences in activity on the day prior to the test may have confounded measured outcomes of appetite.

Participants were asked to fast from 2000 h the previous evening, including liquids, not to exercise in the morning and to arrive at the Unit using motorized transport. At the clinic body weight and waist circumference were measured fasted, and adverse events and/or medications used between treatments recorded. Immediately upon arrival at 7 am the participants were given 200 ml water which they were required to drink in full, after which they were asked to complete baseline VAS rating their subjective feelings of hunger, fullness, satisfaction and prospective food consumption [47]. A venous cannula was also inserted for sequential collection of blood samples throughout the morning, and lipid profile, glucose and insulin concentrations were measured. The HF test breakfast was served at 0830 h and participants were asked to consume the test-meal in full but at their own pace within a 15 minute period. The exact duration of the meal was recorded. No further foods were allowed throughout the morning until an ad lib lunch meal was served 210 minutes later, at 1200 h, to assess energy and macronutrient intake. Lunch was served individually in a quiet dining room with minimal distractions. VAS ratings were measured throughout the morning and for 2 hours after completion of the ad lib lunch. All measurements of food intake were covert. Participants remained at the HNU during each study-day and were allowed to read, write or undertake other such sedentary activities but were not allowed to sleep.

High-fat test breakfasts

The 3 breakfast treatments were HF (50 en% fat) and isoenergetic (2.0 MJ), comprising 2 savoury muffins (ingredients: flour, skimmed milk powder, eggs, tomato, ham, test lipid). The lipids were included as an integral part of the recipe and baked into the muffins. The test breakfast was served at 0830 h, co-presented with 300 ml water. The treatments differed in FA composition and were (i) high-SFA (from butter fat), (ii) high-PUFA (from safflower oil) or (iii) high-MUFA (from olive oil). The energy and macronutrient composition of the 3 test breakfasts was calculated using the dietary program FoodWorks™ (Professional Edition, Version 2.10.136, 1998-2000; Xyris Software) and are shown in Table 1.
Table 1

Energy and macronutrient composition of the 3 high-fat breakfast test meals

 

High-SFA (Butter fat)

High-PUFA (Safflower oil)

High-MUFA (Olive oil)

Weight, as eaten (g)

151

151

145

Energy (kJ)

1974

1952

1955

Energy density (kJ/g)

13.1

12.9

13.4

Total Fat (g)

26.5

25.9

26.1

Protein (g)

15.9

15.7

15.7

CHO (g)

41.9

41.7

41.8

   C12:0

0.8

0

0

   C14:0

2.7

0

0

   C16:0

6.8

1.8

2.9

   C18:0

3.4

0.7

0.8

   other

3.4

0.1

0.2

ΣSFA

17.1

2.6

3.8

   C18:2n6

2.1

19.7

2.4

   C18:3n3

0.2

0.1

0.2

ΣPUFA

2.2

19.8

2.5

   C18:1

6.4

3.7

19.5

   other

0.7

0

0.2

ΣMUFA

7.1

3.7

19.8

kJ from Fat (%)

50.0

49.4

49.4

kJ from Protein (%)

13.9

13.7

13.7

kJ from CHO (%)

34.3

34.2

34.2

Fat as SFA (%)

64.8

10.0

14.6

Fat as PUFA (%)

8.4

75.7

9.6

Fat as MUFA (%)

26.7

14.2

75.8

SFA, saturated fatty acids; PUFA, polyunsaturated fatty acids; MUFA, monounsaturated fatty acids; CHO, carbohydrate

Visual Analogue Scales

Participants rated their hunger, fullness, satisfaction and prospective consumption (how much do you think you could eat now?) using VAS [47]. Subjective feelings were recorded by placing a vertical line across 100 mm scales, anchored at either end by statements; "I am not hungry at all/I am not full at all/I am completely empty/Nothing at all" on the left and "I am as hungry I have ever been/I am totally full/I cannot eat another bite/A large amount" on the right. VAS rating how thirsty, energetic and relaxed the participants felt were included as a distraction from the main outcome. VAS were completed prior to the test meal and 15, 30, 45, 60, 90, 120, 150, 180, 210, [ad lib lunch], 270, 330 and 390 minutes after the participant began consumption of the test breakfast. Immediately after breakfast, participants also rated the pleasantness, visual appeal, smell, taste, aftertaste and overall palatability of the breakfast using separate 100-mm VAS [9, 47]. These questions were anchored on the left by each of the statements "not at all pleasant/bad visual appeal/bad smell/bad taste/strong aftertaste/bad palatability" and on the right by the statements "as pleasant as I have ever tasted/good visual appeal/good smell/good taste/no aftertaste/good palatability".

Ad libitumlunch

The ad libitum lunch consisted of a restricted buffet-style meal with predominantly cold and one hot meal choice, along with a selection of beverages. In an attempt to avoid over-consumption the variation of meal items offered was limited. Participants were advised that they could eat as much or as little as they chose and that they were to remain in the room for a period of 45 minutes. The items presented at the ad lib buffet meal, along with their serving weight, energy and macronutrient content are shown in Table 2. All discrete items (eg bread) were presented as small portions (e.g. quarter slices of bread), and all items were served in excess. Covert weighing of each meal item was carried out before and after lunch to allow calculation of energy and macronutrient intake. Food items were weighed to the nearest 0.5 g (Sartorius AG, Goettingen, Germany), and energy and macronutrient content of foods consumed calculated using the dietary program FoodWorks (Professional Edition Version 2.10.136 1998-2000; Xyris Software).
Table 2

Energy content and macronutrient composition of foods offered to participants at the ad libitum lunch meal

Menu Items

Portion Size (g)

No. of typical serves

Energy (kJ)

Protein (g)

Fat (g)

CHO (g)

Main meal items

      

Fried rice

1600

8

4621

39.0

29.8

167.4

Bread, light rye, quarter slices

168

2

1781

13.3

3.2

62.0

Bread, white, quarter slices

168

2

1814

13.6

3.2

83.8

Chicken breast, flesh, roasted, shredded

190

8

1170

48.1

9.8

0

Smoked ham slices, chopped

190

8

888

30.4

3.8

13.3

Capsicum red & green, raw

68

1

72

0.9

0.2

3.0

Tomatoes, raw

127

1

86

20.0

0.3

3.4

Spiced apple and fruit loaf, quarter slices

400

10

5620

20.0

45.2

212.4

Peach slices, tinned in fruit juice, drained

820

4

1476

4.1

0.8

77.9

Condiments

      

Butter

250

50

6457

2.5

175.0

3.5

Mayonnaise

250

10

3675

2.0

76.8

44.5

Soy sauce

300

10

297

3.0

0

15.0

Drinks

      

Cola drink

1500

10

2943

0

0

178.2

Orange juice

1000

5

1027

2.1

3.1

52.9

Milk, full fat (for decaffein- ated tea/coffee)

1000

20

2550

31.0

33.0

47.0

Water, bottled

1500

10

0

0

0

0

CHO, carbohydrate

Statistical Analysis

VAS data were analysed using a repeated measures Linear Mixed Model ANOVA (SAS: PROC MIXED, SAS version 8.0, SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC, 2001). The participant, the dietary treatment, the intervention period, and the study day were included in the procedure, as was the treatment/time interaction which addressed whether the trajectory over time during the intervention period differed between treatments (diet*time). MANOVA was also used to generate a full model for VAS incorporating all dependent variables. Energy and macronutrient intake data from the ad lib lunch meal was analysed using univariate ANOVA (SAS: PROC MIXED, SAS version 8.0, SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC, 2001). Univariate ANOVA was also used to determine any differences in EI or physical activity level on the day prior to each treatment visit (Day-1). The trial was analysed on the basis of intention to treat (ITT), hence all data from all treatment visits completed were included in the analysis. Missing data was assumed missing at random and no data imputation was performed. Statistical significance was based on 95% limits (P < 0.05). Participant characteristics are presented as mean, standard deviation (mean, SD). Efficacy endpoints are presented as mean, standard error of the mean (mean, SEM).

Results

Participants

Eighteen (18) male participants were randomized into the study, of which 17 completed all 3 treatments. One participant completed only 2 treatments (SFA, MUFA; PUFA treatment was not completed) due to relocation overseas, hence a total of 53 of the scheduled 54 study visits were completed. Participant characteristics are shown in Table 3. They were on average young, lean men. All participants were normoglycaemic with no evidence of hyperlipidaemia or hypertension. On the day prior to intervention there was no significant difference in reported EI (mean, SEM) between treatments (SFA: 8338, 738 kJ/day; PUFA: 8259, 640 kJ/day; MUFA: 7919, 790 kJ/day; treatment, P > 0.05) or reported level of physical activity (treatment, P > 0.05). Physical activity level (mean, SEM) was assessed through hours spent on mild-moderate activity (SFA: 2.1, 0.4 h; PUFA: 2.4, 0.4 h; MUFA: 2.8, 0.6 h), minutes spent on vigorous-strenuous activity (SFA: 4.2, 2.9 m; PUFA: 8.8, 5.6 m; MUFA:10.8, 5.7 m), hours standing (SFA: 4.1, 0.7 h; PUFA: 3.4, 0.5 h; MUFA: 4.0, 0.6 h), hours sitting and watching TV/video (SFA: 4.5, 0.9 h; PUFA: 5.1, 0.8 h; MUFA: 4.2, 0.9 h), and total PEPSA points (SFA:16.9, 4.3; PUFA: 21.2, 6.5; MUFA: 28.1, 10.2). Between participants results were highly variable and there were no significant treatment effects (treatment, P > 0.05).
Table 3

Participant characteristics at baseline.

Baseline

Mean

SD

n

18

 

Age (y)

27.8

8.0

Body weight (kg)

69.3

6.4

BMI (kg/m2)

22.0

1.9

Waist circumference (cm)

75.2

3.9

TC (mmol/L)

4.2

0.7

LDL-C (mmol/L)

2.2

0.5

HDL-C (mmol/L)

1.6

0.3

TAG (mmol/L)

0.9

0.4

TC:HDL-C ratio

2.7

0.5

Glucose (mmol/L)

4.5

0.3

SBP (mm Hg)

108

13

DBP (mm Hg)

64

8

All measurements made at screen visit. Mean (SD). BMI, body mass index; TC, total cholesterol; LDL-C, low density lipoprotein cholesterol; HDL-C, high density lipoprotein cholesterol; TAG, triacylglycerol; SBP, systolic blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure

Visual Analogue Scales

There was no difference in VAS ratings of pleasantness, visual appearance, smell, taste, aftertaste and overall palatability between the 3 HF test breakfasts when assessed immediately following consumption (treatment, P > 0.05, Figure 1), indicating that participants were unable to differentiate between the saturation profiles using visual and sensory cues in this blinded trial. VAS were also used to assess subjective feelings of hunger and satiety throughout the experiment. Figure 2 shows the mean ratings for hunger, fullness, satisfaction and prospective food consumption during the 6 hours following the test breakfast for each of the high-SFA, high-PUFA and high-MUFA treatments. As expected following a 2MJ meal, hunger and prospective consumption significantly decreased and fullness and satisfaction significantly increased immediately following each of the treatments, and then gradually returned to baseline by 210 mins (time, P < 0.01). Consumption of the ad lib lunch also induced significant changes in VAS-rated measures of appetite and satiety during the afternoon. There was however no significant difference in mean ratings of hunger, fullness, satisfaction or prospective consumption between the 3 treatments when measured over the 3.5 hours between the test breakfast and ad lib lunch meal (0-210 mins; all, treatment*time P > 0.05) or when measured throughout the full study day (0-390 mins; all, treatment*time P > 0.05). The use of MANOVA to combine VAS measurements for all treatments was also unable to identify a significant difference between treatments either before lunch or over the full study day (treatment*time, P > 0.05). Analysis of the sequential blood collects also showed there to be no difference between circulating levels of serum cholesterol, triacylglycerol (TAG), glucose or insulin between the SFA, MUFA and PUFA treatments (all, treatment*time P > 0.05).
https://static-content.springer.com/image/art%3A10.1186%2F1475-2891-9-24/MediaObjects/12937_2009_Article_280_Fig1_HTML.jpg
Figure 1

There was no difference between mean (SEM) VAS ratings of pleasantness, visual appeal, smell, taste, aftertaste or palatability between the 3 test-breakfasts when assessed immediately after the meal (treatment, P > 0.05). SFA, high-saturated fatty acids (n = 18); PUFA, high-polyunsaturated fatty acids (n = 17), MUFA, high-monounsaturated fatty acids (n = 18).

https://static-content.springer.com/image/art%3A10.1186%2F1475-2891-9-24/MediaObjects/12937_2009_Article_280_Fig2_HTML.jpg
Figure 2

Mean (SEM) VAS for hunger, fullness, satisfaction and prospective consumption between 0-390 minutes. Each high lipid test breakfast was given immediately after the baseline (t = 0 mins) VAS, and the ad libitum lunch was served at 210 minutes. SFA, high-saturated fatty acids (n = 18); PUFA, high-polyunsaturated fatty acids (n = 17), MUFA, high-monounsaturated fatty acids (n = 18).

Energy and macronutrient intake at the ad libitumlunch

Mean total EI and energy contributed by CHO, fat and protein respectively at the ad lib lunch is presented for each treatment in Figure 3. There was no significant difference in total EI between lipid treatments (treatment, P > 0.05). Mean (SEM) EI at lunch was 5275.9 (286.5) kJ, 5227.7 (403.9) kJ, and 5215.6 (329.5) kJ following the SFA-, PUFA-, and MUFA-rich breakfasts respectively. The maximum difference between treatments was less than 2%. When macronutrient intake was compared between treatments there was no significant difference in CHO, fat or protein nor weight of food consumed at the lunch meal (treatment, P > 0.05).
https://static-content.springer.com/image/art%3A10.1186%2F1475-2891-9-24/MediaObjects/12937_2009_Article_280_Fig3_HTML.jpg
Figure 3

Mean (SEM) energy intake at the ad libitum lunch meal, showing the macronutrient components consumed. SFA, high-saturated fatty acids (n = 18); PUFA, high-polyunsaturated fatty acids (n = 17), MUFA, high-monounsaturated fatty acids (n = 18).

Discussion

There was no evidence from this study that changes made to the fatty acid saturation of a high fat breakfast altered subjective hunger ratings or energy intake at the subsequent lunch meal in a group of healthy male participants. This is consistent with a number [4143] but not all [3638, 40] previously published studies, and there are several issues concerning study design which it is important to consider.

There are a wide range of published methods by which short-term appetite regulation has been assessed but consensus is yet to be achieved as to best practice [48]. The methodology in our current trial was based primarily on the lipid emulsion trials of Burns and colleagues [4951] where small manipulations in lipids at a test breakfast induced significant changes in EI at a subsequent lunch meal. Arguably, the long separation between the intervention and the ad lib lunch (>3 hours) in these trials may make changes in eating behaviour more difficult to effect than studies where an outcome meal is given only 60 or 90 minutes following the test treatment. In our current trial however the ad lib energy intake was consistent with the recorded postprandial feelings of hunger and fullness which also did not differ across lipid treatments. It is possible that the varied item buffet-style lunch may have encouraged overconsumption, decreased sensitivity and masked differential effects, but again it is notable that a multi-item buffet lunch has previously been shown to be sensitive to effects of acute lipid manipulations [4951]. We have previously shown it to be sensitive to changes in energy content at a breakfast meal [44]. Pleasantness, visual appeal, smell, taste, after-taste and palatability were all well matched between treatments in our current trial, and differences in the energy density of the test lipids were small.

There is little consensus as to whether degree of lipid saturation may play a role in regulation of body weight and adiposity through mechanisms driving changes in either intake or oxidation. Whilst epidemiological evidence of an association between lipid saturation and adiposity in studies such as the Quebec Family Study [24] suggest that saturation may influence either intake or oxidation, evidence remains scant. Friedman originally proposed the hypothesis that oxidation of macronutrients following a meal may exert a negative feedback on appetite and eating behavior, and hence control the satiating power of a given nutrient [18]. Whilst human studies have been unable confirm this mechanism [17], there are some data which do show an oxidative gradient may exist and may be driven in part by FA saturation [25, 2733] and hence the hypothesis remains of interest [21]. Interestingly, FA saturation has also been shown to impact postprandial hormone response including the peptides insulin [52] and CCK [34, 36] which may drive changes in intake, although again the data remains inconclusive [5355].

Much of the evidence for effect of saturation on EI has been derived from acute studies of intake of similar design to our current trial and also from acute GI infusion trials, several of which show increased satiety effects of PUFA. Intestinal infusion of Intralipid (54% PUFA linoleic acid, 29% MUFA oleic acid) and PUFA (linoleic acid), but not SFA (stearic acid) or MUFA (oleic acid) emulsions tended to decrease EI relative to a saline control in a study of men given a restricted lunch meal [36]. The effects were modest however with no significant differential effects between the 4 fat loaded treatments. This was supported in part by Lawton et al (2000) in a CCL-matched feeding study which demonstrated decreased ratings of appetite and a trend, albeit non-significant, towards lower ad lib EI over the rest of the day following a high-PUFA (linoleic acid) and SFA (stearic-oleic blend) lunch meal compared with a lunch rich in MUFA (oleic acid) [37]. A second study by the same authors was unable to confirm these results, although merging data from the 2 studies did support the greater satiety effects of PUFA [37]. A more recent intraduodenal infusion study of the C12:0 SFA lauric acid versus longer chain C18:1 MUFA oleic acid showed a decrease in EI following the SFA but not MUFA treatment relative to a saline control [38]. Conversely, there are also studies which find MUFA to be more satiating than PUFA [39, 40]. For example, Burton-Freeman et al (2005) found that mean VAS scores of appetite satisfaction were greater following a high-oleic safflower oil (high-MUFA) compared with a walnut oil (high-PUFA) preload, although it is notable that this result was not substantiated by 4 other appetite-related VAS or subsequent ad lib EI at lunch [39]. Finally there are further trials which are consistent with our current study and which have been unable to detect a difference in ratings of appetite or EI between meals of varying saturation profile [4143]. It is apparent that a clear effect of lipid saturation on satiety has yet to be established.

Some variability in outcome may in part be due to differences in methods. Important aspects of trials assessing postprandial changes in appetite may include number and characteristics of participants; lipid dose, composition and route of administration; inter-meal interval; and composition and variety of foods offered at the ad lib outcome meal. These have been summarized in Table 4. Of the trials so far conducted, all were small sample studies (8-25 participants) conducted predominantly in lean, healthy participants. Dose of lipid administered does not appear to unduly bias outcome since studies by Lawton [37] and Flint [42] both administered high doses of lipid (>50 g fat), yet only the Lawton study conducted in lean participants showed differential effects of FA saturation. The fact that delivery of lower doses, such as our current trial (26 g), are sufficient to elicit a response was demonstrated by Kamphuis and colleagues where a 25 g lipid supplement differentially altered EI [40]. Burton-Freeman [39] administered a low dose (9-13 g) lipid treatment yet observed significant effects on some VAS-assessed satiety ratings, albeit these did not result in suppression of EI at a subsequent test meal. The route of lipid administration is likely to also be important and it is notable that 2 [38, 56] of the 4 [37, 40] trials where a differential response was elicited delivered the lipid treatments via GI infusion hence bypassing early sensory and cognitive signaling. Whilst it is difficult to find methodological issues directly responsible for the variable outcome of the studies to date, each of the trials appears robust in its approach, it is clear that a strong case cannot be built for enhanced satiety with high-SFA, MUFA or PUFA based on current evidence.
Table 4

Previous studies investigating the effect of fatty acid saturation on subjective appetite ratings and ad libitum energy intake (measured or from diet records) at a meal, highlighting methodological differences between trials

Publication

Participants

Lipid dose

Lipid composition [% of total lipid]

Inter-meal interval

Study endpoint

Study outcome

Current study: Strik et al., 2010

Lean, men, n = 18

26 g, [50 en% fat]

Butter fat, high stearic-lauric blend [65% SFA]; Olive oil, high oleic acid [76% MUFA]; Safflower oil, linoleic acid [76% PUFA]

210 mins

Ad libitum EI from lunch meal

No effect of saturation on EI

Lawton et al., 2000 [37]

Lean, men, n = 10 women, n = 10

Women 58 g; Men 83 g; [55 en% fat]

Stearic-oleic blend [44% SFA, 44% MUFA]; High-oleic oil [81% MUFA]; High linoleic oil [75% PUFA]

240 mins

Ad libitum EI from buffet dinner + snack boxes

PUFA and SFA tended to decrease EI relative to MUFA (trend only)

French et al.,2000 [36]

Lean, men, n = 10

Duodenal infusion; 20 g lipid emulsion [100 en% fat], at rate of 1 mL/min over 100 mins

High stearic/oleic [40% SFA; 44% MUFA]; High oleic acid [75% MUFA]; High linoleic acid [74% PUFA]; Intralipid [16% SFA, 29% MUFA, 54% PUFA]; saline

90 mins

Ad libitum EI from lunch meal

PUFA decreased EI relative to SFA and MUFA (trend only); signif lower than saline control

Kamphuis et al., 2001 [40]

Overweight, men, n = 8; women, n = 8

20 ml (~20 g) lunch; 25 ml (~25 g) dinner added to usual diet [~36 en % fat]

High-linoleic oil [67% LA, PUFA]; High-γ-linolenic [20% GLA, PUFA]; High-oleic oil [80% MUFA]

Test lipids given as part of ad lib meals

24-h ad libitum EI: restricted choice; lunch, dinner, snacks

MUFA decreased EI relative to PUFA at test dinner; no difference over 24 h

Alfenas et al., 2003 [43]

Lean, men, n = 9; women, n = 11

30-40 g; [54-59 en % fat]

Butter fat [66% SFA]; Peanut oil [49% MUFA]; Canola oil [62% MUFA]; fat free

>120 mins

Diet records of EI during free feeding over 24-h [no outcome meal]

No effect of saturation on EI

MacIntosh et al., 2003 [41]

Lean, men, n = 10

30 g; [55 en% fat]

Butter fat [69% SFA]; Sunola oil [80% MUFA]; Sunflower oil [64% PUFA];

120 mins

Ad libitum EI from lunch meal + diet; diet records over rest of day

No effect of saturation on EI

Flint et al., 2003 [42]

Overweight men, n = 19

63-87 g; [60 en% fat]

High-oleic sunflower oil [83% MUFA]; Hydrogenated rapeseed oil [54% trans; 31% SFA]; Grape- seed oil [70% PUFA]

300 mins

Ad libitum EI from lunch meal

No effect of saturation on EI

Burton-Freeman et al., 2005 [39]

Lean, men, n = 12; women, n = 13

Men 13 g; Women 9 g; [39 en% fat]

High-oleic safflower oil [72% MUFA]; Walnut oil [66% PUFA];Ground walnuts [66% PUFA]; low fat [1.4 g fat]

45 mins

Ad libitum EI from lunch meal

No effect of saturation on EI

Feltrin et al. 2008 [38]

Lean, men, n = 13

Duodenal infusion; ~3 g lipid emulsion [100en% fat], at rate of 4 mL/min over 60 mins

Lauric acid [100% SFA]; Oleic acid [100% MUFA]; saline

60 mins

Ad libitum EI from lunch meal

SFA decreased EI relative to MUFA and saline control

Conclusions

In this study we were unable to show differential changes in postprandial feelings of hunger or fullness, or changes in energy intake at a lunch meal when alterations were made to the fatty acid saturation of a high-fat breakfast. Lipid dose, the interval between test-breakfast and ad libitum outcome lunch, and the composition of the ad lib meal have all been identified as areas which may bias outcome. In this study we administered a 26 g lipid test meal which was well matched for sensory outcomes between treatments, 3.5 hours prior to a free choice lunch meal. Whether a higher dose lipid product administered as a preload rather than test meal, i.e. closer to the lunch meal, would elicit an appetite response is unknown. Certainly there is little consensus on effects of dietary lipid composition on appetite control from previous studies. Whether appetite may be altered by longer term and sustained changes in dietary fatty acid composition also remains to be demonstrated.

List of abbreviations used

ANOVA: 

analysis of variance

BMI: 

body mass index

CCL: 

carbon chain length

CCK: 

cholecystokinin

CHO: 

carbohydrate

EI: 

energy intake

FA: 

fatty acid

HF: 

high fat

HNU: 

human nutrition unit

MANOVA: 

multivariate analysis of variance

MUFA: 

monounsaturated fatty acid

PEPSA: 

Physical Exercise Programme for Sedentary Adults

PUFA: 

polyunsaturated fatty acid

SD: 

standard deviation

SEM: 

standard error of the mean

SFA: 

saturated fatty acid

VAS: 

visual analogue score.

Declarations

Acknowledgements

LactoPharma New Zealand provided the funding for this trial. We thank Glyn Muir, Shelley Baty and Nisha Patel who provided technical assistance during the study. We also thank the participants in this intervention trial.

Authors’ Affiliations

(1)
Human Nutrition Unit, University of Auckland
(2)
Trinity College
(3)
Human Nutrition Unit & School of Public Health, University of Auckland
(4)
Fonterra Research Centre
(5)
Department of Statistics, University of Auckland
(6)
Human Nutrition Unit, School of Biological Sciences & Department of Medicine, University of Auckland

References

  1. Lissner L, Heitmann BL: Dietary fat and obesity: evidence from epidemiology. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1995, 49 (2): 79-90.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bray GA, Paeratakul S, Popkin BM: Dietary fat and obesity: a review of animal, clinical and epidemiological studies. Physiol Behav. 2004, 83 (4): 549-555. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.08.039.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Willett WC: Dietary fat and obesity: an unconvincing relation. Am J Clin Nutr. 1998, 68 (6): 1149-1150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Hill JO, Astrup A: What diets should we be recommending for obesity?. Obesity Reviews. 2003, 4 (2): 77-78. 10.1046/j.1467-789X.2003.00103.x.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Astrup A: Carbohydrates as macronutrients in relation to protein and fat for body weight control. Int J Obes. 2006, 30 (S3): S4-S9. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803485.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  6. Astrup A, Grunwald GK, Melanson EL, Saris WH, Hill JO: The role of low-fat diets in body weight control: a meta-analysis of ad libitum dietary intervention studies. Int J Obes. 2000, 24 (12): 1545-1552. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801453.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  7. Pirozzo S, Summerbell C, Cameron C, Glasziou P: Should we recommend low-fat diets for obesity?. Obesity Reviews. 2003, 4 (2): 83-90. 10.1046/j.1467-789X.2003.00099.x.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Field AE, Willett WC, Lissner L, Colditz GA: Dietary fat and weight gain among women in the Nurses' Health Study. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007, 15 (4): 967-976. 10.1038/oby.2007.616.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  9. Rolls BJ, Kim-Harris S, Fischman MW, Foltin RW, Moran TH, Stoner SA: Satiety after preloads with different amounts of fat and carbohydrate: implications for obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 1994, 60 (4): 476-487.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Blundell JE, Burley VJ, Cotton JR, Lawton CL: Dietary fat and the control of energy intake: evaluating the effects of fat on meal size and postmeal satiety. Am J Clin Nutr. 1993, 57 (5 Suppl): 772S-777S. discussion 777S-778SPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Lawton CL, Burley VJ, Wales JK, Blundell JE: Dietary fat and appetite control in obese subjects: weak effects on satiation and satiety. Int J Obes. 1993, 17 (7): 409-416.Google Scholar
  12. Poppitt SD, McCormack D, Buffenstein R: Short-term effects of macronutrient preloads on appetite and energy intake in lean women. Physiol Behav. 1998, 64 (3): 279-285. 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00061-4.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. French S, Robinson T: Fats and food intake. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2003, 6: 629-634. 10.1097/00075197-200311000-00004.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Stubbs RJ, Whybrow S: Energy density, diet composition and palatability: influences on overall food energy intake in humans. Physiol Behav. 2004, 81: 755-764. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.027.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Poppitt SD, Prentice AM: Energy density and its role in the control of food intake: evidence from metabolic and community studies. Appetite. 1996, 26 (2): 153-174. 10.1006/appe.1996.0013.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS: Reductions in portion size and energy density of foods are additive and lead to sustained decreases in energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006, 83: 11-17.PubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Raben A, Agerholm-Larsen L, Flint A, Holst JJ, Astrup A: Meals with similar energy densities but rich in protein, fat, carbohydrate, or alcohol have different effects on energy expenditure and substrate metabolism but not on appetite and energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003, 77: 91-100.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Friedman MI: Fuel partitioning and food intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 1998, 67 (3): 513S-518.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Friedman MI, Tordoff MG, Ramirez I: Integrated metabolic control of food intake. Brain Res Bull. 1986, 17 (6): 855-859. 10.1016/0361-9230(86)90099-7.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Arvaniti K, Richard D, Tremblay A: Reproducibility of energy and macronutrient intake and related substrate oxidation rates in a buffet-type meal. Br J Nutr. 2000, 83 (5): 489-495.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Langhans W: Fatty acid oxidation in the energostatic control of eating - A new idea. Appetite. 2008, 51: 446-451. 10.1016/j.appet.2008.06.014.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Monteleone P, Bencivenga R, Longobardi N, Serritella C, Maj M: Differential responses of circulating ghrelin to high-fat or high-carbohydrate meal in healthy women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2003, 88: 5510-5514. 10.1210/jc.2003-030797.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Pedersen-Bjergaard U, Høst U, Kelbaek H, Schifter S, Rehfeld JF, Faber J, Christensen NJ: Influence of meal composition on postprandial peripheral plasma concentrations of vasoactive peptides in man. Scand J Clin Lab Invest. 1996, 56: 497-503. 10.3109/00365519609088805.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Doucet E, Almeras N, White MD, Despres JP, Bouchard C, Tremblay A: Dietary fat composition and human adiposity. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1998, 52 (1): 2-6. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600500.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Piers LS, Walker KZ, Stoney RM, Soares MJ, O'Dea K: Substitution of saturated with monounsaturated fat in a 4-week diet affects body weight and composition of overweight and obese men. Br J Nutr. 2003, 90 (3): 717-727. 10.1079/BJN2003948.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Storlien LH, Hulbert AJ, Else PL: Polyunsaturated fatty acids, membrane function and metabolic diseases such as diabetes and obesity. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 1998, 1 (6): 559-563. 10.1097/00075197-199811000-00014.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Jones PJ, Schoeller DA: Polyunsaturated:saturated ratio of diet fat influences energy substrate utilization in the human. Metabolism. 1988, 37 (2): 145-151. 10.1016/S0026-0495(98)90009-9.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Jones PJ, Ridgen JE, Phang PT, Birmingham CL: Influence of dietary fat polyunsaturated to saturated ratio on energy substrate utilization in obesity. Metabolism. 1992, 41 (4): 396-401. 10.1016/0026-0495(92)90074-K.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Piers LS, Walker KZ, Stoney RM, Soares MJ, O'Dea K: The influence of the type of dietary fat on postprandial fat oxidation rates: monounsaturated (olive oil) vs saturated fat (cream). Int J Obes. 2002, 26 (6): 814-821. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801993.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  30. Soares MJ, Cummings SJ, Mamo JC, Kenrick M, Piers LS: The acute effects of olive oil v. cream on postprandial thermogenesis and substrate oxidation in postmenopausal women. Br J Nutr. 2004, 91 (2): 245-252. 10.1079/BJN20031047.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. DeLany JP, Windhauser MM, Champagne CM, Bray GA: Differential oxidation of individual dietary fatty acids in humans. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000, 72 (4): 905-911.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Jones PJ, Pencharz PB, Clandinin MT: Whole body oxidation of dietary fatty acids: implications for energy utilization. Am J Clin Nutr. 1985, 42 (5): 769-777.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Kien CL, Bunn JY, Ugrasbul F: Increasing dietary palmitic acid decreases fat oxidation and daily energy expenditure. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005, 82 (2): 320-326.PubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Beardshall K, Frost G, Morarji Y, Domin J, Bloom SR, Calam J: Saturation of fat and cholecystokinin release: implications for pancreatic carcinogenesis. Lancet. 1989, 2 (8670): 1008-1010. 10.1016/S0140-6736(89)91017-9.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Mullen BJ, Martin RJ: The effect of dietary fat on diet selection may involve central serotonin. Am J Physiol. 1992, 263 (3 Pt 2): R559-563.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. French SJ, Conlon CA, Mutuma ST, Arnold M, Read NW, Meijer G, Francis J: The effects of intestinal infusion of long-chain fatty acids on food intake in humans. Gastroenterol. 2000, 119 (4): 943-948. 10.1053/gast.2000.18139.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  37. Lawton CL, Delargy HJ, Brockman J, Smith FC, Blundell JE: The degree of saturation of fatty acids influences post-ingestive satiety. Br J Nutr. 2000, 83 (5): 473-482.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Feltrin KL, Little TJ, Meyer JH, Horowitz M, Rades T, Wishart J, Feinle-Bisset C: Comparative effects of intraduodenal infusions of lauric and oleic acids on antropyloroduodenal motility, plasma cholecystokinin and peptide YY, appetite, and energy intake in healthy men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008, 87: 1181-1187.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Burton-Freeman B: Sex and cognitive dietary restraint influence cholecystokinin release and satiety in response to preloads varying in fatty acid composition and content. J Nutr. 2005, 135 (6): 1407-1414.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Kamphuis MM, Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Saris WH: Fat-specific satiety in humans for fat high in linoleic acid vs fat high in oleic acid. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2001, 55 (6): 499-508. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601222.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. MacIntosh CG, Holt SH, Brand-Miller JC: The degree of fat saturation does not alter glycemic, insulinemic or satiety responses to a starchy staple in healthy men. J Nutr. 2003, 133 (8): 2577-2580.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Flint A, Helt B, Raben A, Toubro S, Astrup A: Effects of different dietary fat types on postprandial appetite and energy expenditure. Obes Res. 2003, 11 (12): 1449-1455. 10.1038/oby.2003.194.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Alfenas RC, Mattes RD: Effect of fat sources on satiety. Obesity Res. 2003, 11 (2): 183-187. 10.1038/oby.2003.29.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  44. Lithander FE, Strik CM, McGill AT, MacGibbon AK, McArdle BH, Poppitt SD: No effect of an oleoylethanolamide-related phospholipid on satiety and energy intake: a randomised controlled trial of phosphatidylethanolamine. Lipids Health Dis. 2008.Google Scholar
  45. Stunkard AJ, Messick S: The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res. 1985, 29 (1): 71-83. 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Grant B, Wilson N: PEPSA Diary. 1993, Dunedin: PEPSA LtdGoogle Scholar
  47. Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A: Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes. 2000, 24 (1): 38-48. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801083.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  48. Blundell JE, deGraaf C, Hulshof T, Jebb S, Livingstone B, Lluch A, Mela D, Salah S, Schuring E, VanderKnaap H, et al: Appetite control: methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes Rev. 2010,Google Scholar
  49. Burns AA, Livingstone MB, Welch RW, Dunne A, Robson P, Lindmark L, Reid CA, Mullaney U, Rowland IR: Short-term effects of yoghurt containing a novel fat emulsion on energy and macronutrient intakes in non-obese subjects. Int J Obes. 2000, 24: 1419-1425. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801430.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  50. Burns AA, Livingstone MB, Welch RW, Dunne A, Reid CA, Rowland IR: The effects of yoghurt containing a novel fat emulsion on energy and macronutrient intakes in non-overweight, overweight and obese subjects. J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2001, 25: 1487-1496. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801720.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  51. Burns AA, Livingstone MB, Welch RW, Dunne A, Rowland IR: Dose-response effects of a novel fat emulsion (Olibra) on energy and macronutrient intakes up to 36 h post-consumption. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2002, 56: 368-377. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601326.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Thomsen C, Rasmussen O, Lousen T, Holst JJ, Fenselau S, Schrezenmeir J, Hermansen K: Differential effects of saturated and monounsaturated fatty acids on postprandial lipemia and incretin responses in healthy subjects. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999, 69 (6): 1135-1143.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Poppitt SD, Keogh GF, Mulvey TB, Phillips A, McArdle BH, MacGibbon AK, Cooper GJ: Effect of moderate changes in dietary fatty acid profile on postprandial lipaemia, haemostatic and related CVD risk factors in healthy men. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2004, 58: 819-827. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601882.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Blundell JE: Serotonin and the biology of feeding. Am J Clin Nutr. 1992, 55 (1): 155S-159.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Flint A, Gregersen NT, Gluud LL, Møller BK, Raben A, Tetens I, Verdich C, Astrup A: Associations between postprandial insulin and blood glucose responses, appetite sensations and energy intake in normal weight and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis of test meal studies. Br J Nutr. 2007, 98: 17-25. 10.1017/S000711450768297X.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. French S: Effects of dietary fat and carbohydrate on appetite vary depending upon site and structure. Br J Nutr. 2004, 92 (Suppl 1): S23-26. 10.1079/BJN20041137.View ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright

© Strik et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. 2010

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.